Relations between Pavlovian-instrumental transfer and reinforcer devaluation.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] W. Estes. Discriminative conditioning. I. A discriminative property of conditioned anticipation. , 1943 .
[2] R. Rescorla,et al. Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. , 1967, Psychological review.
[3] José M. R. Delgado,et al. Integrative Activity of the Brain , 1968, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.
[4] D. R. Thomas,et al. Information value and stimulus configuring as factors in conditioned reinforcement. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[5] N. Azrin,et al. Positive conditioned suppression: conditioned suppression using positive reinforcers as the unconditioned stimuli. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[6] Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning in the rat. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[7] Heith Cd,et al. Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning in the rat. , 1973 .
[8] D. Bindra. A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. , 1974, Psychological review.
[9] W. K. Honig,et al. Handbook of Operant Behavior , 2022 .
[10] J. Karpicke. Directed approach responses and positive conditioned suppression in the rat , 1978 .
[11] P. Lovibond. Effects of long- and variable-duration signals for food on activity, instrumental responding, and eating , 1980 .
[12] J. E. Sherman,et al. Aversion to the reinforcer differentially affects conditioned reinforcement and instrumental responding. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[13] P. Lovibond. Appetitive Pavlovian-Instrumental Interactions: Effects of Inter-Stimulus Interval and Baseline Reinforcement Conditions , 1981, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[14] Christopher D. Adams. Variations in the Sensitivity of Instrumental Responding to Reinforcer Devaluation , 1982 .
[15] A. Dickinson,et al. Irrelevant Incentive Learning during Instrumental Conditioning: The Role of the Drive-Reinforcer and Response-Reinforcer Relationships , 1983 .
[16] Christopher D. Adams,et al. The Effect of the Instrumental Training Contingency on Susceptibility to Reinforcer Devaluation , 1983 .
[17] A. Dickinson,et al. Irrelevant Incentive Learning during Training on Ratio and Interval Schedules , 1983 .
[18] H. Weingarten. Conditioned cues elicit feeding in sated rats: a role for learning in meal initiation. , 1983, Science.
[19] R. Rescorla,et al. Instrumental responding remains sensitive to reinforcer devaluation after extensive training , 1985 .
[20] M. Schlossberg. Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms. , 1986 .
[21] A. Dickinson,et al. Pavlovian Processes in the Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance , 1987 .
[22] R. Rescorla. Pavlovian conditioning. It's not what you think it is. , 1988, The American psychologist.
[23] R. Rescorla,et al. Associations between the discriminative stimulus and the reinforcer in instrumental learning. , 1988 .
[24] Effect of reinforcer devaluation on discriminative control of instrumental behavior. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[25] R. Rescorla. Instrumental responses become associated with reinforcers that differ in one feature , 1990 .
[26] P. Holland. Event representation in Pavlovian conditioning: Image and action , 1990, Cognition.
[27] D. Weisz,et al. An associative process maintains reflex facilitation of the unconditioned nictitating membrane response during the early stages of training. , 1990, Behavioral neuroscience.
[28] Modulation of a discrete Pavlovian conditioned reflex by a putative emotive Pavlovian conditioned stimulus. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[29] R. Rescorla. Response-outcome versus outcome-response associations in instrumental learning , 1992 .
[30] R. Rescorla. Transfer of instrumental control mediated by a devalued outcome , 1994 .
[31] R. Colwill,et al. Encoding of the unconditioned stimulus in Pavlovian conditioning , 1994 .
[32] R. Boakes,et al. Motivational control after extended instrumental training , 1995 .
[33] B. Balleine,et al. Motivational Control of Instrumental Action , 1995 .
[34] P. Holland,et al. Neurotoxic Lesions of Basolateral, But Not Central, Amygdala Interfere with Pavlovian Second-Order Conditioning and Reinforcer Devaluation Effects , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[35] P. Holland. Brain mechanisms for changes in processing of conditioned stimuli in Pavlovian conditioning: Implications for behavior theory , 1997 .
[36] K. Berridge,et al. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.
[37] K. Lattal,et al. Trial and intertrial durations in Pavlovian conditioning: issues of learning and performance. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[38] J. Mirenowicz,et al. Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.
[39] P. Holland. Trial and intertrial durations in appetitive conditioning in rats , 2000 .
[40] C. Gallistel,et al. Time, rate, and conditioning. , 2000, Psychological review.
[41] Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick,et al. Independent effects of stimulus and cycle duration in conditioning: The role of timing processes , 2000 .
[42] A. Dickinson,et al. Involvement of the central nucleus of the amygdala and nucleus accumbens core in mediating Pavlovian influences on instrumental behaviour , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.
[43] B. Balleine,et al. The Role of the Nucleus Accumbens in Instrumental Conditioning: Evidence of a Functional Dissociation between Accumbens Core and Shell , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] G. Hall,et al. Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[45] P. Holland,et al. The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.
[46] S. S. Stevens,et al. Learning, motivation, and emotion , 2002 .
[47] R. Colwill,et al. Instrumental responding remains under the control of the consequent outcome after extended training , 2002, Behavioural Processes.
[48] J. N. P. Rawlins,et al. Effects of cytotoxic nucleus accumbens lesions on instrumental conditioning in rats , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] B. Balleine,et al. The Role of Learning in the Operation of Motivational Systems , 2002 .
[50] P. Holland,et al. Double dissociation of the effects of lesions of basolateral and central amygdala on conditioned stimulus‐potentiated feeding and Pavlovian‐instrumental transfer , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[51] B. Balleine,et al. Instrumental and Pavlovian incentive processes have dissociable effects on components of a heterogeneous instrumental chain. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.