N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control.

Event-related potentials were recorded in a flanker task using arrowheads pointing to the left or to the right as targets and as congruent or incongruent flanker stimuli using squares as neutral flanker stimuli. The onset of the flanker stimuli preceded that of the target stimuli by 100 ms. Lateralized readiness potentials showed response activation below execution threshold in correspondence to the information conveyed by the flanker stimuli. Exclusively, the incongruent flanker condition provoked a N2c, which evolved closely synchronized to the erroneous response. Graded response analyses separating incongruent trials with weak, medium, and strong incorrect response activation revealed that the N2c amplitude covaried with the magnitude of the erroneous response. The N2c in the incongruent compatibility condition of the flanker task thus corresponds to the avoidance of inappropriate responses, possibly reflecting the inhibition of automatically but erroneously primed responses. The results are compatible with studies of error correction, suggesting that efference monitoring is a constituent of executive control.

[1]  Joseph R. Higgins,et al.  Correction of false moves in pursuit tracking , 1969 .

[2]  J H Flowers,et al.  The effect of flanking context on visual classification: The joint contribution of interactions at different processing levels , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[4]  R. Angel Efference copy in the control of movement , 1976, Neurology.

[5]  E. Jodo,et al.  Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[6]  P. M. A. RABBITT,et al.  Error Correction Time without External Error Signals , 1966, Nature.

[7]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  ERPs to stimuli requiring response production and inhibition: effects of age, probability and visual noise. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  E. Donchin,et al.  Probability effects on stimulus evaluation and response processes. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[11]  M Giray,et al.  Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  R W Angel,et al.  Correction of tracking errors without sensory feedback. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  P. Berg,et al.  The residual after correcting event-related potentials for blink artifacts. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[14]  E. Schröger Event-related potentials to auditory stimuli following transient shifts of spatial attention in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.

[15]  J. Ford,et al.  ERPs to response production and inhibition. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  O. Neumann Visual Attention and Action , 1990 .

[17]  J. H. Flowers,et al.  Priming effects in perceptual classification , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[19]  David A. Taylor Time Course of Context Effects. , 1977 .

[20]  E. Donchin,et al.  Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials , 1980, Brain Research.

[21]  A. Kok Effects of degradation of visual stimuli on components of the event-related potential (ERP) in go/nogo reaction tasks , 1986, Biological Psychology.

[22]  B. Kopp,et al.  Error-correcting behavior in schizophrenic patients , 1994, Schizophrenia Research.

[23]  C. Eriksen,et al.  A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[25]  C W Eriksen,et al.  Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Virginia A. Diggles,et al.  Rapid error correction during human arm movements: evidence for central monitoring. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  James D. St. James Observations on the microstructure of response conflict. , 1990 .

[28]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  G Mulder,et al.  Selective response activation can begin before stimulus recognition is complete: a psychophysiological and error analysis of continuous flow. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[31]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[32]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  C. B. Gibbs PROBABILITY LEARNING IN STEP-INPUT TRACKING. , 1965, British journal of psychology.

[34]  P M Rabbitt,et al.  Three Kinds of Error-Signalling Responses in a Serial Choice Task , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  E. Donchin,et al.  Detecting early communication: Using measures of movement-related potentials to illuminate human information processing , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[36]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[39]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[40]  R W Angel,et al.  Tracking errors amended without visual feedback. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.

[42]  E. D. Megaw,et al.  Directional Errors and Their Correction in a Discrete Tracking Task , 1972 .

[43]  G. Grice,et al.  Temporal characteristics of noise conditions producing facilitation and interference , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  G. Grice,et al.  Temporal dynamics of associative interference and facilitation produced by visual context , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.