Selective inhibition of a multicomponent response can be achieved without cost.

Behavioral flexibility frequently requires the ability to modify an on-going action. In some situations, optimal performance requires modifying some components of an on-going action without interrupting other components of that action. This form of control has been studied with the selective stop-signal task, in which participants are instructed to abort only one movement of a multicomponent response. Previous studies have shown a transient disruption of the nonaborted component, suggesting limitations in our ability to use selective inhibition. This cost has been attributed to a structural limitation associated with the recruitment of a cortico-basal ganglia pathway that allows for the rapid inhibition of action but operates in a relatively generic manner. Using a model-based approach, we demonstrate that, with a modest amount of training and highly compatible stimulus-response mappings, people can perform a selective-stop task without any cost on the nonaborted component. Prior reports of behavioral costs in selective-stop tasks reflect, at least in part, a sampling bias in the method commonly used to estimate such costs. These results suggest that inhibition can be selectively controlled and present a challenge for models of inhibitory control that posit the operation of generic processes.

[1]  A. Aron,et al.  Stop the Presses , 2008, Psychological science.

[2]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Responding with Restraint: What Are the Neurocognitive Mechanisms? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  E. Waters,et al.  The ontogenesis of smiling and laughter: a perspective on the organization of development in infancy. , 1976, Psychological review.

[5]  Nitin Tandon,et al.  Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[6]  A. Aron,et al.  Proactive Selective Response Suppression Is Implemented via the Basal Ganglia , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Having a goal to stop action is associated with advance control of specific motor representations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  G. Logan,et al.  The Development of Selective Inhibitory Control Across the Life Span , 2002, Developmental neuropsychology.

[9]  A. Aron,et al.  Stimulation at dorsal and ventral electrode contacts targeted at the subthalamic nucleus has different effects on motor and emotion functions in Parkinson's disease , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  J. Duncan Response Selection Errors in Spatial Choice Reaction Tasks , 1977 .

[11]  M Hallett,et al.  The Time Course of Changes in Motor Cortex Excitability Associated with Voluntary Movement , 1999, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[12]  M. W. Molen,et al.  Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses , 2004 .

[13]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Retrieval Processes in Recognition Memory , 1976 .

[14]  Nitin Tandon,et al.  Intracranial electroencephalography reveals different temporal profiles for dorsal- and ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex in preparing to stop action. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[16]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Jennifer M. Glass,et al.  Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.

[18]  W. Byblow,et al.  Selective inhibition of movement. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  M. Inase,et al.  Dual somatotopical representations in the primate subthalamic nucleus: evidence for ordered but reversed body-map transformations from the primary motor cortex and the supplementary motor area , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Aging and inhibition: beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[21]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  D. Meyer,et al.  The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[25]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Selective stopping? Maybe not. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Jobi S. George,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals dissociable mechanisms for global versus selective corticomotor suppression underlying the stopping of action. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  Caitlin L. Oldenkamp,et al.  A Proactive Mechanism for Selective Suppression of Response Tendencies , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  R. Ratcliff Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. , 1966 .

[31]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[32]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  How to use MATLAB to fit the ex-Gaussian and other probability functions to a distribution of response times , 2008 .

[35]  Adam R. Aron,et al.  Stopping speech suppresses the task-irrelevant hand , 2012, Brain and Language.

[36]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Haptic tracking permits bimanual independence. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  Jan R. Wessel,et al.  Chronometric Electrical Stimulation of Right Inferior Frontal Cortex Increases Motor Braking , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  S Kornblum,et al.  Does motor programming necessitate response execution? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and Stroop task. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[40]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[42]  T. Mima,et al.  Suppression of human cortico-motoneuronal excitability during the Stop-signal task , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[43]  Michael A. DiSano,et al.  Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  The psychological refractory period effect following callosotomy: uncoupling of lateralized response codes. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  Hayley J. MacDonald,et al.  Uncoupling response inhibition. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Ian Greenhouse,et al.  Stopping a response has global or nonglobal effects on the motor system depending on preparation. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  A. Parent,et al.  Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. II. The place of subthalamic nucleus and external pallidium in basal ganglia circuitry , 1995, Brain Research Reviews.

[48]  Jan R Wessel,et al.  Unexpected Events Induce Motor Slowing via a Brain Mechanism for Action-Stopping with Global Suppressive Effects , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.