Impact of distracting activities and drivers' cognitive failures on driving performance

The rapid increase in the availability of smart phones and other infotainment devices, and their widespread use while driving, contributes significantly to car crash rates. This is since the human brain has limited capacity and cannot perform two tasks at the same time, but rather switches from one to another rapidly. Understanding of the impact of distracting activities on drivers’ behavior is essential in order to support the development of effective technology and policy solutions to mitigate its potential risk. The main objective of this study is to investigate the impact of several distracting activities, drivers’ characteristics, their reported frequency of involvement in distracting activities, and cognitive failures on driving performance. A driving simulator experiment was developed to collect data on several driver performance measures while undertaking different distracting activities. 101 volunteer drivers participated in the study (68 males and 33 females) with age range between 18 to 57 years old. Each driver drove four scenarios on two-lane rural highway, while undertaking various activities: (1) using a hand-held cell-phone; (2) texting; (3) eating; and (4) a control scenario (no distracting activity). In all of these scenarios the distracting activity started with the beginning of the scenario and lasted till the scenario ended. Each scenario took on average 4 minutes. During the scenarios the speeds of the other vehicles in front of the subject varied between 20-100 km/h, and their acceleration and decelerations varied between 0.4-2.5 m/sec2. The speeds and accelerations were chosen randomly from pre-defined uniform distributions. Data on the longitudinal and lateral movements of the vehicles were recorded in the experiment and various measures were calculated and extracted from this raw data. In addition, all participants completed a questionnaire on their personal characteristics, their frequency of involvement in distracting activities and level of distractibility, and the “The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire”. Significant differences were found in the driving performance measures among the distracting activities. In particular, texting had the largest negative effect on driving performance. It increased the variability in driving speeds, lateral deviations and the distance gaps from the lead vehicle. Similar but milder effects were also found for the other two distracting activities. Gender effect was small and insignificant, except in the lateral deviations, in which males had lower values. Compared to the older drivers, younger drivers (<26 years old) kept significantly shorter distances from their leader and had better lateral control (lower lateral deviations). The reported frequency of involvement in distracting activities while driving was not found to significantly affect driving performance. In contrast, drivers’ level of distractibility had a significant impact on driving speeds and their variability, indicating heterogeneity among drivers with respect to their vulnerability to distracting activities. The results of this study show that distracting activities negatively impact driving performance for both genders and all age groups, regardless of their experience in performing a second task while driving.

[1]  John D. Lee,et al.  Driver Distraction : Theory, Effects, and Mitigation , 2008 .

[2]  Shlomo Bekhor,et al.  Association of risk proneness in overtaking maneuvers with impaired decision making , 2008 .

[3]  D. Strayer,et al.  Cell phone-induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[4]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Examining the Impact of Cell Phone Conversations on Driving Using Meta-Analytic Techniques , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[5]  John D. Lee,et al.  Defining Driver Distraction , 2009 .

[6]  D Solomon,et al.  Accidents on main rural highways related to speed, driver, and vehicle , 1964 .

[7]  Harry Zhang,et al.  Identification of Real-Time Diagnostic Measures of Visual Distraction With an Automatic Eye-Tracking System , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[8]  Marco Dozza,et al.  What factors influence drivers' response time for evasive maneuvers in real traffic? , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[9]  Dot Hs The Impact of Driver Inattention On Near-Crash/Crash Risk: , 2006 .

[10]  G C Staughton,et al.  METHODOLOGY OF AN IN-DEPTH ACCIDENT INVESTIGATION SURVEY , 1977 .

[11]  Crystal A. Franklin,et al.  Impact of distracted driving on safety and traffic flow. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[12]  Jeff K. Caird,et al.  A Research Synthesis of Text Messaging and Driving Performance , 2017 .

[13]  D. Broadbent,et al.  The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. , 1982, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[14]  Thomas J Triggs,et al.  Driver distraction: the effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age on driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[15]  W. C. Maples,et al.  The effects of cell phone use on peripheral vision. , 2008, Optometry.

[16]  Yulan Liang,et al.  Driver performance effects of simultaneous visual and cognitive distraction and adaptation behavior , 2012 .

[17]  Neil K Chaudhary,et al.  District of Columbia. , 1896, The Journal of comparative medicine and veterinary archives.

[18]  Ingrid van Schagen,et al.  Driving speed and the risk of road crashes: a review. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[19]  Jeff K. Caird,et al.  Effects of Passenger and Cellular Phone Conversations on Driver Distraction , 2004 .

[20]  Shlomo Bekhor,et al.  Risk evaluation by modeling of passing behavior on two-lane rural highways. , 2009, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[21]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone , 2001, Psychological science.

[22]  Frank A. Drews,et al.  Passenger and Cell-Phone Conversations in Simulated Driving , 2004 .

[23]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Multiple resources and performance prediction , 2002 .

[24]  D. Strayer,et al.  Cell-Phone–Induced Driver Distraction , 2007 .

[25]  Rebecca L Olson,et al.  The Impact of Hand-Held and Hands-Free Cell Phone Use on Driving Performance and Safety-Critical Event Risk , 2013 .

[26]  Paul Atchley,et al.  Conversation Limits the Functional Field of View , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[27]  Cory Siebe,et al.  Distracted Driving and Risk of Road Crashes among Novice and Experienced Drivers , 2014 .

[28]  Joel M. Cooper,et al.  Drivers ’ Lane Changing Behavior While Conversing On a Cell Phone in a Variable Density Simulated Highway Environment , .

[29]  David B. Kaber,et al.  Situation awareness and workload in driving while using adaptive cruise control and a cell phone , 2005 .

[30]  Katja Kircher,et al.  Driver distraction : a review of the literature , 2007 .

[31]  Richard J. Hanowski,et al.  Driver Distraction in Commercial Vehicle Operations , 2009 .

[32]  David Shinar,et al.  The tendency of drivers to pass other vehicles , 2005 .

[33]  Jeff K Caird,et al.  A meta-analysis of the effects of cell phones on driver performance. , 2008, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[34]  A. Lenhart,et al.  Teens and Mobile Phones: Text Messaging Explodes as Teens Embrace It as the Centerpiece of Their Communication Strategies with Friends. , 2010 .

[35]  Mark Vollrath,et al.  The effect of visual and cognitive distraction on driver’s anticipation in a simulated car following scenario , 2011 .

[36]  Karel Brookhuis,et al.  Issues arising from the HASTE experiments , 2005 .