Two- and three-stimuli auditory oddball ERP tasks and neuropsychological measures in aging

The purpose of this study was to decide whether the P300 from an auditory three-stimuli oddball task is more sensitive to aging than the P300 from a two-stimuli task, and to investigate the relationship between neuropsychological tests and the different ways of eliciting the P300. Thirty-one adults (21.8–94.7 years) completed five neuropsychological tests and two ERP tasks. The way P300 was elicited did not influence its relationship to aging or neuropsychological scores, and the correlations between the two paradigms were strong for amplitude, but rather weak for latency. The main conclusion is that the P300 from an auditory three-stimulus paradigm is sensitive to aging and neuropsychologically valid.

[1]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[2]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[3]  G Fein,et al.  P300 latency variability in normal elderly: effects of paradigm and measurement technique. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  Clinical utility of long latency 'cognitive' event-related potentials (P3): the cons. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[5]  A. Shimamura Memory and amnesia. , 1990, The Western journal of medicine.

[6]  B. O’Donnell,et al.  Active and passive P3 latency and psychometric performance: influence of age and individual differences. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[7]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Effects of aging on event-related brain potentials and reaction times in an auditory oddball task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[8]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 in young and elderly subjects: auditory frequency and intensity effects. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  R. Zec,et al.  The neuropsychology of aging , 1995, Experimental Gerontology.

[10]  J. Polich,et al.  Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[11]  J. Polich,et al.  Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[12]  J. Polich,et al.  P3a, perceptual distinctiveness, and stimulus modality. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  S. Lane,et al.  Laboratory and psychometric measurements of impulsivity among violent and nonviolent female parolees , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  R Näätänen,et al.  Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[15]  J. Polich,et al.  Auditory and visual P300 topography from a 3 stimulus paradigm , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[16]  S. Kobayashi,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in response to novel sounds in dementia , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[17]  Denise C. Park,et al.  The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. , 2000 .

[18]  J. Polich,et al.  Auditory P3a assessment of male alcoholics , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[19]  Paavo Alku,et al.  Increased distractibility in closed head injury as revealed by event‐related potentials , 2000, Neuroreport.