Of goals and habits: age-related and individual differences in goal-directed decision-making

In this study we investigated age-related and individual differences in habitual (model-free) and goal-directed (model-based) decision-making. Specifically, we were interested in three questions. First, does age affect the balance between model-based and model-free decision mechanisms? Second, are these age-related changes due to age differences in working memory (WM) capacity? Third, can model-based behavior be affected by manipulating the distinctiveness of the reward value of choice options? To answer these questions we used a two-stage Markov decision task in in combination with computational modeling to dissociate model-based and model-free decision mechanisms. To affect model-based behavior in this task we manipulated the distinctiveness of reward probabilities of choice options. The results show age-related deficits in model-based decision-making, which are particularly pronounced if unexpected reward indicates the need for a shift in decision strategy. In this situation younger adults explore the task structure, whereas older adults show perseverative behavior. Consistent with previous findings, these results indicate that older adults have deficits in the representation and updating of expected reward value. We also observed substantial individual differences in model-based behavior. In younger adults high WM capacity is associated with greater model-based behavior and this effect is further elevated when reward probabilities are more distinct. However, in older adults we found no effect of WM capacity. Moreover, age differences in model-based behavior remained statistically significant, even after controlling for WM capacity. Thus, factors other than decline in WM, such as deficits in the in the integration of expected reward value into strategic decisions may contribute to the observed impairments in model-based behavior in older adults.

[1]  J. Stevens,et al.  Animal Intelligence , 1883, Nature.

[2]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[3]  H. Harman,et al.  Kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests , 1976 .

[4]  Learning to Choose. , 1981 .

[5]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Effects of adult age and working memory on reasoning and spatial abilities. , 1989, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.

[6]  R. Engle,et al.  Is working memory capacity task dependent , 1989 .

[7]  C. Carver,et al.  Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales , 1994 .

[8]  Timothy A. Salthouse,et al.  The aging of working memory. , 1994 .

[9]  Mahesan Niranjan,et al.  On-line Q-learning using connectionist systems , 1994 .

[10]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: A computational model , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  A computational account of altered error processing in older age: Dopamine and the error-related negativity , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Computational roles for dopamine in behavioural control , 2004, Nature.

[14]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Transformations in the Couplings Among Intellectual Abilities and Constituent Cognitive Processes Across the Life Span , 2004, Psychological science.

[15]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  An automated version of the operation span task , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[16]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[17]  P. Glimcher,et al.  JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 84, 555–579 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) DYNAMIC RESPONSE-BY-RESPONSE MODELS OF MATCHING BEHAVIOR IN RHESUS MONKEYS , 2022 .

[18]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  On the Control of Control: The Role of Dopamine in Regulating Prefrontal Function and Working Memory , 2007 .

[19]  B. Eppinger,et al.  Better or worse than expected? Aging, learning, and the ERN , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Learning to Avoid in Older Age , 2008 .

[21]  Y. Niv,et al.  Dialogues on prediction errors , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  BOLD Responses Reflecting Dopaminergic Signals in the Human Ventral Tegmental Area , 2008, Science.

[23]  Derick H. Lindquist,et al.  Neuronal Basis of Learning , 2009 .

[24]  J. Rieskamp,et al.  Learning to choose: Cognitive aging and strategy selection learning in decision making. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[25]  P. Dayan,et al.  States versus Rewards: Dissociable Neural Prediction Error Signals Underlying Model-Based and Model-Free Reinforcement Learning , 2010, Neuron.

[26]  Camelia M. Kuhnen,et al.  Variability in Nucleus Accumbens Activity Mediates Age-Related Suboptimal Financial Risk Taking , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  B. Balleine,et al.  Human and Rodent Homologies in Action Control: Corticostriatal Determinants of Goal-Directed and Habitual Action , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[28]  Darrell A. Worthy,et al.  With Age Comes Wisdom , 2011, Psychological science.

[29]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Neuromodulation of reward‐based learning and decision making in human aging , 2011, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Viktor Müller,et al.  Life Span Differences in Electrophysiological Correlates of Monitoring Gains and Losses during Probabilistic Reinforcement Learning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  P. Dayan,et al.  Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors , 2011, Neuron.

[32]  Jutta Kray,et al.  To Choose or to Avoid: Age Differences in Learning from Positive and Negative Feedback , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  T. Endrass,et al.  Aging, probabilistic learning and performance monitoring , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[34]  N. Daw,et al.  The ubiquity of model-based reinforcement learning , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  D. Hämmerer,et al.  Dopaminergic and prefrontal contributions to reward-based learning and outcome monitoring during child development and aging. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[36]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Role of prefrontal cortex and the midbrain dopamine system in working memory updating , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  Darrell A. Worthy,et al.  Age-Based Differences in Strategy Use in Choice Tasks , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[38]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Frontostriatal White Matter Integrity Mediates Adult Age Differences in Probabilistic Reward Learning , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  P. Dayan,et al.  Neural Prediction Errors Reveal a Risk-Sensitive Reinforcement-Learning Process in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  J. Duncan,et al.  Task rules, working memory, and fluid intelligence , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  P. Dayan,et al.  Dopamine restores reward prediction errors in old age , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  A. Markman,et al.  The Curse of Planning: Dissecting Multiple Reinforcement-Learning Systems by Taxing the Central Executive , 2013 .

[43]  Nicolas W. Schuck,et al.  Reduced Striatal Responses to Reward Prediction Errors in Older Compared with Younger Adults , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Margaret Mackey Learning to Choose , 2014 .

[45]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[46]  N. McGlynn Thinking fast and slow. , 2014, Australian veterinary journal.

[47]  A. Markman,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General Retrospective Revaluation in Sequential Decision Making : A Tale of Two Systems , 2012 .