Food-associated calls as public information about patch quality
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Evidence for a “monitoring call” in red‐bellied tamarins , 1990, American journal of primatology.
[2] P. Marler,et al. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): I. Socioecological factors , 1993 .
[3] P. Tannenbaum,et al. Food-associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins , 1991, Animal Behaviour.
[4] P. Marler,et al. Vocal communication in the domestic chicken: II. Is a sender sensitive to the presence and nature of a receiver? , 1986, Animal Behaviour.
[5] T. Valone,et al. Patch estimation by group foragers: what information is used? , 1993, Animal Behaviour.
[6] J. Krebs. Colonial Nesting and Social Feeding as Strategies for Exploiting Food Resources in the Great Blue Heron (Ardea Herodias) , 1974 .
[7] D. Winkler,et al. Flock-feeding on fish schools increases individual success in gulls , 1986, Nature.
[8] M. Hauser,et al. Manipulation of food calls in captive chimpanzees. A preliminary report. , 1987, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.
[9] K. Stewart. Gorillas : variation in female relationships , 1987 .
[10] T. Caraco,et al. Living in groups: is there an optimal group size? , 1984 .
[11] J. McNamara. Optimal patch use in a stochastic environment , 1982 .
[12] T. Caraco,et al. Co-operative and non-co-operative bases of food-calling , 1989 .
[13] C. Clark,et al. The evolutionary advantages of group foraging , 1986 .
[14] Jennifer J. Templeton,et al. Patch assessment in foraging flocks of European starlings: evidence for the use of public information , 1995 .
[15] Thomas Caraco,et al. Risk‐Sensitivity and Foraging Groups , 1981 .
[16] N. Caine,et al. Factors affecting the rates of food calls given by red-bellied tamarins , 1995, Animal Behaviour.
[17] T. Valone. Group foraging, public information, and patch estimation , 1989 .
[18] S. L. Lima. Protective cover and the use of space : different strategies in finches , 1990 .
[19] L. Lefebvre,et al. Manipulating foraging group size: spider monkey food calls at fruiting trees , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[20] J. Krebs,et al. Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach , 1978 .
[21] M. Elgar. House sparrows establish foraging flocks by giving chirrup calls if the resources are divisible , 1986, Animal Behaviour.
[22] S. L. Lima,et al. Vigilance while feeding and its relation to the risk of predation , 1987 .
[23] P. Marler,et al. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression , 1993 .
[24] A. Stokes,et al. COURTSHIP FEEDING IN GALLINACEOUS BIRDS , 1971 .
[25] S. Boinski. Vocal coordination of troop movement among white‐faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus , 1993, American journal of primatology.
[26] J. J. Benz. Food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins: design features for representational communication , 1993, Animal Behaviour.
[27] Foraging on patches: are groups disadvantaged? , 1995 .
[28] C. Clark,et al. Foraging and Flocking Strategies: Information in an Uncertain Environment , 1984, The American Naturalist.
[29] Richard F. Green,et al. Stochastic Models of Optimal Foraging , 1987 .
[30] E. Ranta,et al. Competition Versus Cooperation: Success of Individuals Foraging Alone and in Groups , 1993, The American Naturalist.
[31] Thomas J. Valone,et al. Patch Information and Estimation: A Cost of Group Foraging , 1993 .
[32] R. J. F. Smith,et al. Evolution of Alarm Signals: Role of Benefits of Retaining Group Members or Territorial Neighbors , 1986, The American Naturalist.
[33] J. Parrish. Re-examining the selfish herd: are central fish safer? , 1989, Animal Behaviour.
[34] E. Greene. Individuals in an osprey colony discriminate between high and low quality information , 1987, Nature.
[35] W. Hamilton. Geometry for the selfish herd. , 1971, Journal of theoretical biology.