Stochastic processes in postural center-of-pressure profiles

The stochastic processes of postural center-of-pressure profiles were examined in 3- and 5-year-old children, young adult students (mean 20 years), and an elderly age group (mean 67 years). Subjects stood still in an upright bipedal stance on a force platform under vision and nonvision conditions. The time evolutionary properties of the center-of-pressure dynamic were examined using basic stochastic process models. The amount of motion of the center of pressure decreased with increments of age from 3 to 5 years to young adult but increased again in the elderly age group. The availability of vision decreased the amount of motion of the center of pressure in all groups except the 3-year-old group, where there was less motion of the center of pressure with no vision. The stochastic properties of the center-of-pressure dynamic were assessed using both a two-process, random-walk model of Collins and De Luca and an Ornstein-Uhlenbeck model that is linear and has displacement governed only by a single stiffness term in the random walk. The two-process open- and closed-loop model accounted for about 96% and the Ornstein-Uhlenbeck model 92% of the variance of the diffusion term. Diffusion parameters in both models showed that the data were correlated and that they varied with age in a fashion consistent with developmental accounts of the changing regulation of the degrees of freedom in action. The findings suggest that it is premature to consider the trajectory of the center-of-pressure as a two-process, open- and closed-loop random-walk model given that: (a) the linear Ornstein-Uhlenbeck dynamic equation with only two parameters accommodates almost as much of the variance of the random walk; and (b) the linkage of a discontinuity in the diffusion process with the transition of open- to closed-loop processes is poorly founded. It appears that the nature of the stochastic properties of the random walk of the center-of-pressure trajectory in quiet, upright standing remains to be elucidated.

[1]  S. A. Riedel,et al.  Standing postural stability assessment and signal stationarity in children with cerebral palsy , 1993 .

[2]  J. Collins,et al.  Open-loop and closed-loop control of posture: A random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajectories , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Short-term non-stationarity and the development of postural control , 1997 .

[4]  J. J. Collins,et al.  The effects of visual input on open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  J. Collins,et al.  Random walking during quiet standing. , 1994, Physical review letters.

[6]  K. Newell,et al.  Aging and time to instability in posture. , 1998, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[7]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Postural dynamics as a function of skill level and task constraints , 1994 .

[8]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  On postural stability and variability , 1993 .

[9]  A. Seireg,et al.  Normal postural stability and steadiness: quantitative assessment. , 1975, The Journal of bone and joint surgery. American volume.

[10]  K M Newell,et al.  Quantification of postural sway patterns in tardive dyskinesia , 1993, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[11]  M. Woollacott,et al.  Neuromuscular control of posture in the infant and child: is vision dominant? , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  Jacek Cholewicki,et al.  The effects of visual input on postural control of the lumbar spine in unstable sitting. , 2003, Human movement science.

[13]  J. J. Collins,et al.  Age-related changes in open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  W Freedman,et al.  Nonstationary properties of postural sway. , 1993, Journal of biomechanics.

[15]  J. Sheldon On the Natural History of Falls in Old Age* , 1960, British medical journal.

[16]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Chapter 1 Visual Processing Time and the Control of Movement , 1992 .

[17]  J. Judge,et al.  Functional base of support decreases with age. , 1994, Journal of gerontology.

[18]  P. Goldie,et al.  Force platform measures for evaluating postural control: reliability and validity. , 1989, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[19]  R. Schmidt Control Processes In Motor Skills , 1976, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[20]  David A. Winter,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement , 1990 .

[21]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Learning to Coordinate Redundant Biomechanical Degrees of Freedom , 1994 .

[22]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants , 1974 .

[23]  C. Gardiner Handbook of Stochastic Methods , 1983 .

[24]  G. Powell,et al.  Power spectral density analysis of lateral human standing sway. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Dynamics of posture in 3- and 5-year-old children as a function of task constraints , 1994 .

[26]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[27]  H. Berg Random Walks in Biology , 2018 .

[28]  K. Hayes,et al.  Maturation Of Postural Sway In Young Children , 1987, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[29]  A. Goldberger,et al.  Loss of 'complexity' and aging. Potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. , 1992, JAMA.