The computation of contingency in classical conditioning

Publisher Summary This chapter discusses a unified framework, which encompasses the computations, algorithms, and neurobiological implementations underlying classical conditioning. It presents an extensive mathematical analysis of the constraints on classical conditioning—that is, the precise contingency conditions under which mammals may and may not learn a particular association between two events in a classical conditioning situation. In classical conditioning, an unconditional stimulus (US)—that is, a cue, which is inherently biologically salient to an animal (such as an electric shock), is repeatedly paired with a conditional stimulus (CS), a cue that initially has no special significance to the animal over repeated trials, the animal can learn that the CS is predictive of or associated with the US. This phenomenon of associative learning is subject to laws and constraints: An association is learned to some extent in some conditions and to a lesser extent in others.

[1]  B. Skyrms Choice and chance : an introduction to inductive logic , 1968 .

[2]  Richard F. Thompson,et al.  Modeling the Neural Substrates of Associative Learning and Memory: A Computational Approach , 1987 .

[3]  R. Rescorla Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  E. Kandel,et al.  Is there a cell-biological alphabet for simple forms of learning? , 1984 .

[5]  R D FITZGERALD,et al.  EFFECTS OF PARTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH ACID ON THE CLASSICALLY CONDITIONED SALIVARY RESPONSE IN DOGS. , 1963, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Volume 423. Timing and Time Perception Held at New York on 10-13 May 1983, , 1984 .

[7]  A. R. Wagner,et al.  REINFORCEMENT HISTORY AND THE EXTINCTION OF A CONDITIONED SALIVARY RESPONSE. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[8]  E Gamzu,et al.  Classical Conditioning of a Complex Skeletal Response , 1971, Science.

[9]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[10]  K. Spence The nature of discrimination learning in animals. , 1936 .

[11]  D R Shanks Continuous monitoring of human contingency judgment across trials , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[12]  R. Rescorla Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. , 1967, Psychological review.

[13]  Judea Pearl,et al.  Reverend Bayes on Inference Engines: A Distributed Hierarchical Approach , 1982, AAAI.

[14]  R. Rescorla Informational Variables in Pavlovian Conditioning , 1972 .

[15]  R. Rescorla Predictability and number of pairings in Pavlovian fear conditioning , 1966 .

[16]  A. Gelperin,et al.  Rapid taste-aversion learning by an isolated molluscan central nervous system. , 1980, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Frank Rosenblatt,et al.  PRINCIPLES OF NEURODYNAMICS. PERCEPTRONS AND THE THEORY OF BRAIN MECHANISMS , 1963 .

[18]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Timing and time perception. , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[19]  L. J. Hammond A traditional demonstration of the active properties of Pavlovian inhibition using differential CER , 1967 .

[20]  L. Kamin Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning , 1967 .

[21]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[22]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[23]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Contingency spaces and measures in classical and instrumental conditioning. , 1974, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[24]  N. Mackintosh A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .

[25]  M. Domjan,et al.  Backward conditoning as an inhibitory procedure , 1971 .

[26]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980 .

[27]  Partial reinforcement and the CER , 1966 .

[28]  A G Barto,et al.  Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. , 1981, Psychological review.

[29]  E. Hearst,et al.  Positive and negative relations between a signal and food: Approach-withdrawal behavior to the signal. , 1977 .

[30]  Edward A. Wasserman,et al.  Perception of causal relations in humans: Factors affecting judgments of response-outcome contingencies under free-operant procedures☆ , 1983 .

[31]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[32]  P. W. Frey,et al.  Inhibition and learning , 1973 .

[33]  E. Kandel,et al.  Effects of interstimulus interval and contingency on classical conditioning of the Aplysia siphon withdrawal reflex , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  Richard Granger,et al.  Contingency and latency in associative learning : computational, algorithmic and implementation analyses , 1985 .

[35]  S. Grossberg Processing of Expected and Unexpected Events During Conditioning and Attention: A Psychophysiological Theory , 1982 .

[36]  H. M. Jenkins,et al.  The Judgment of Contingency and the Nature of the Response Alternatives , 1980 .

[37]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[38]  Stephen A. Ritz,et al.  Distinctive features, categorical perception, and probability learning: some applications of a neural model , 1977 .

[39]  A. R. Wagner,et al.  PARTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF THE CLASSICALLY CONDITIONED EYELID RESPONSE IN THE RABBIT. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.