High-speed navigators: Using more than what meets the eye.

This study employed a novel method to dissociate the use of external visual information and internal spatial representations in human navigation. Using a goal-directed walking task and gaze-contingent displays, 14 participants with normal vision navigated within an immersive virtual forest during which each participant's field of view (FOV) was restricted to 10, 20, or 40 deg in diameter. Participants were classified into two groups, good and poor navigators, based on a cluster analysis of their individual mean latencies, walk times, and path efficiencies in the 10 deg condition. Changes in performance measures across the three FOVs were calculated for the two groups. Significant interactions were found, with the overall performance of the poor navigators decreasing at a faster rate than the performance of the good navigators. Perceptual spans were also calculated for the two groups, and it was determined that the good navigators were able to complete the same task as effectively as the poor navigators with a smaller FOV. Collectively, these results support recent theories stating that good navigators rely on internal spatial representations to a greater extent than poor navigators do.

[1]  E. Spelke,et al.  Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .

[2]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[3]  M. Hollands,et al.  Coordination of Eye and Leg Movements During Visually Guided Stepping , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  M. Blades,et al.  Mapping as a Cultural and Cognitive Universal , 2003 .

[5]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Eye movements and the integration of visual memory and visual perception , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  Paul E. Downing,et al.  Viewpoint-Specific Scene Representations in Human Parahippocampal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[7]  G K Aguirre,et al.  Neural components of topographical representation. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Where we Go With a Little Good Information , 1999 .

[9]  Helga C. Arsenio,et al.  Panoramic search: the interaction of memory and vision in search through a familiar scene. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Yoshiaki Shirai,et al.  Look where you're going [robotic wheelchair] , 2003, IEEE Robotics Autom. Mag..

[11]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.

[12]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions , 1997 .

[13]  David E. Irwin Information integration across saccadic eye movements , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  Kathleen A. Turano,et al.  Optic-flow and egocentric-direction strategies in walking: Central vs peripheral visual field , 2005, Vision Research.

[15]  C I Howarth,et al.  The efficiency and walking speed of visually impaired people. , 1986, Ergonomics.

[16]  H. Harman,et al.  Kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests , 1976 .

[17]  J. Lovie-Kitchin,et al.  Measurements of preferred walking speed in subjects with central and peripheral vision loss , 2004, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[18]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[19]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[20]  Simon Ungar,et al.  Cognitive mapping without visual experience , 2018, Cognitive Mapping.

[21]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? , 2001, Vision Research.

[22]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.

[23]  R. Kitchin,et al.  Cognitive mapping , 2018, Cognitive Mapping.

[24]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Covert shifts of attention precede involuntary eye movements. , 2010, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[26]  N. Charness,et al.  Visual Span in Expert Chess Players: Evidence From Eye Movements , 2001, Psychological science.

[27]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Real-time simulation of arbitrary visual fields , 2002, ETRA.

[28]  William H. Warren,et al.  Perception of heading is a brain in the neck , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  David N. Lee,et al.  Sensory and intrinsic coordination of movement , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[31]  M. Pomplun,et al.  Investigating the visual span in comparative search: the effects of task difficulty and divided attention , 2001, Cognition.

[32]  E. Spelke,et al.  Updating egocentric representations in human navigation , 2000, Cognition.

[33]  S. Asfour,et al.  Discrete wavelet transform: a tool in smoothing kinematic data. , 1999, Journal of biomechanics.

[34]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Optic flow and scene structure do not always contribute to the control of human walking , 2002, Vision Research.

[35]  Gordon E Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading XX. Linking letter recognition to reading speed in central and peripheral vision , 2001, Vision Research.

[36]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[37]  K. Turano,et al.  Oculomotor strategies for the direction of gaze tested with a real-world activity , 2003, Vision Research.

[38]  J E Cutting,et al.  Heading and path information from retinal flow in naturalistic environments , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  A E Patla,et al.  Where and when do we look as we approach and step over an obstacle in the travel path? , 1997, Neuroreport.

[41]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Navigation expertise and the human hippocampus: A structural brain imaging analysis , 2003, Hippocampus.

[42]  M. Land,et al.  The Roles of Vision and Eye Movements in the Control of Activities of Daily Living , 1998, Perception.

[43]  David N. Lee,et al.  Where we look when we steer , 1994, Nature.

[44]  Michael F. Land,et al.  Predictable eye-head coordination during driving , 1992, Nature.

[45]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Development of Visually Guided Locomotion , 1998 .

[46]  D. Regan,et al.  Visual flow and direction of locomotion. , 1985, Science.

[47]  J. Rieser,et al.  How Persons with Visual Impairments Explore Novel Spaces: Strategies of Good and Poor Performers , 1993 .

[48]  K. Rayner Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[49]  J E Cutting,et al.  Wayfinding, displacements, and mental maps: velocity fields are not typically used to determine one's aimpoint. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  David N. Lee Guiding Movement by Coupling Taus , 1998 .

[51]  J. Gibson The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement. , 1994, Psychological review.

[52]  J. H. Bertera,et al.  Eye movements and the span of the effective stimulus in visual search , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[53]  C. C. Trowbridge ON FUNDAMENTAL METHODS OF ORIENTATION AND "IMAGINARY MAPS". , 1913, Science.

[54]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  The spatial brain. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[55]  J E Cutting,et al.  Visual flow and direction of locomotion. , 1985, Science.

[56]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[57]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Spatial memory and saccadic targeting in a natural task. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[58]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[59]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[60]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[61]  D. Ballard,et al.  Memory Representations in Natural Tasks , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  A. Patla,et al.  “Look where you’re going!”: gaze behaviour associated with maintaining and changing the direction of locomotion , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading—XVI. The visual span in normal and low vision , 1997, Vision Research.

[64]  R. D. Calkins Orientation of Maps , 1909 .

[65]  J. Gibson The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement. 1954. , 1994, Psychological review.

[66]  S Saida,et al.  Useful visual field size for pattern perception , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[67]  John A. King,et al.  Memory for events and their spatial context: models and experiments. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[68]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[69]  T. McNamara,et al.  Orientation and perspective dependence in route and survey learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[70]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual Guidance of Intercepting a Moving Target on Foot , 2004, Perception.

[71]  Geoffrey Underwood,et al.  Restricting the field of view to investigate the perceptual spans of pianists , 2003 .