Human tactile detection of within- and inter-finger spatiotemporal phase shifts of low-frequency vibrations

When we touch an object, the skin copies its surface shape/texture, and this deformation pattern shifts according to the objects movement. This shift pattern directly encodes spatio-temporal “motion” information of the event, and has been detected in other modalities (e.g., inter-aural time differences for audition and first-order motion for vision). Since previous studies suggested that mechanoreceptor-afferent channels with small receptive field and slow temporal characteristics contribute to tactile motion perception, we tried to tap the spatio-temporal processor using low-frequency sine-waves as primitive probes in our previous study. However, we found that asynchrony of sine-wave pair presented on adjacent fingers was difficult to detect. Here, to take advantage of the small receptive field, we investigated within-finger motion and found above threshold performance when observers touched localized sine-wave stimuli with one finger. Though observers could not perceptually discriminate rightward from leftward motion, the adaptation occurred in a direction-sensitive way: the motion/asynchronous detection was impaired by adapting to asynchronous stimuli moving in the same direction. These findings are consistent with a possibility that human can directly encode short-range spatio-temporal patterns of skin deformation by using phase-shifted low-frequency components, in addition to detecting short- and long-range motion using energy shifts of high-frequency components.

[1]  M. Rowe,et al.  Synaptic transmission between single slowly adapting type I fibres and their cuneate target neurones in cat. , 1994, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  James C Craig,et al.  Convergence of submodality-specific input onto neurons in primary somatosensory cortex. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. Nicholls,et al.  A left hemisphere, but not right hemispace, advantage for tactual simultaneity judgments , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  J. DiCarlo,et al.  Spatial and Temporal Structure of Receptive Fields in Primate Somatosensory Area 3b: Effects of Stimulus Scanning Direction and Orientation , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  E P Gardner,et al.  Simulation of motion on the skin. I. Receptive fields and temporal frequency coding by cutaneous mechanoreceptors of OPTACON pulses delivered to the hand. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  George A. Gescheider,et al.  Vibrotactile masking: Effects of oneand two-site stimulation , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  R. Johansson,et al.  Responses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin displacements , 1982, Brain Research.

[8]  Hannes P. Saal,et al.  Millisecond Precision Spike Timing Shapes Tactile Perception , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[10]  D. Ginty,et al.  The Sensory Neurons of Touch , 2013, Neuron.

[11]  O. Franzén,et al.  Somesthesis and the Neurobiology of the Somatosensory Cortex , 1996, Advances in Life Sciences.

[12]  D. Ferrington,et al.  Differential contributions to coding of cutaneous vibratory information by cortical somatosensory areas I and II. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  O. Braddick A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.

[14]  G. Geffen,et al.  Effects of Preferred Hand and Sex on the Perception of Tactile Simultaneity , 2000, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[15]  Junji Watanabe,et al.  Somatotopic dominance in tactile temporal processing , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Heidi S. Fisher,et al.  Adaptation from invisible flicker. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  E. P. Gardner,et al.  A quantitative analysis of responses of direction-sensitive neurons in somatosensory cortex of awake monkeys. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Hannes P. Saal,et al.  Rate and timing of cortical responses driven by separate sensory channels , 2015, eLife.

[19]  S J Bensmaïa,et al.  Complex tactile waveform discrimination. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  J. Craig,et al.  Temporal order and tactile patterns , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  S. Nishida,et al.  A common perceptual temporal limit of binding synchronous inputs across different sensory attributes and modalities , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  J. Randall Flanagan,et al.  Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  L. Thurstone PSYCHOPHYSICAL ANALYSIS , 2008 .

[24]  Diego Miranda-Saavedra,et al.  Chromerid genomes reveal the evolutionary path from photosynthetic algae to obligate intracellular parasites , 2015, eLife.

[25]  K. O. Johnson,et al.  Surround suppression in the responses of primate SA1 and RA mechanoreceptive afferents mapped with a probe array. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  M. Tommerdahl,et al.  Human vibrotactile frequency discriminative capacity after adaptation to 25 Hz or 200 Hz stimulation , 2005, Brain Research.

[27]  Gian Domenico Iannetti,et al.  A Fovea for Pain at the Fingertips , 2013, Current Biology.

[28]  M. Koslowsky,et al.  tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: a meta-analytical review , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  B L Whitsel,et al.  The response of SI directionally selective neurons to stimulus motion occurring at two sites within the receptive field. , 1993, Somatosensory & motor research.

[30]  K O Johnson,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial tactile discrimination: neural patterns evoked by braille‐like dot patterns in the monkey. , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[31]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Task effects on tactile temporal order judgments: when space does and does not matter. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  J. Hyvärinen,et al.  Cortical neuronal mechanisms in flutter-vibration studied in unanesthetized monkeys. Neuronal periodicity and frequency discrimination. , 1969, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  L A JEFFRESS,et al.  A place theory of sound localization. , 1948, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[34]  Ernst Pöppel,et al.  Mindworks: Time and Conscious Experience , 1988 .

[35]  E P Gardner,et al.  Temporal integration of multiple-point stimuli in primary somatosensory cortical receptive fields of alert monkeys. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  E P Gardner,et al.  Spatial integration of multiple-point stimuli in primary somatosensory cortical receptive fields of alert monkeys. , 1980, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[37]  J. Craig,et al.  Shape Invariant Coding of Motion Direction in Somatosensory Cortex , 2010, PLoS biology.

[38]  Susumu Tachi,et al.  Directional remapping in tactile inter-finger apparent motion: a motion aftereffect study , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  M. J. Rowe,et al.  Corticothalamic influences on transmission of tactile information in the ventroposterolateral thalamus of the cat: effect of reversible inactivation of somatosensory cortical areas I and II , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Junji Watanabe,et al.  Neural timing signal for precise tactile timing judgments. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  The sense of flutter-vibration: comparison of the human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from the monkey hand. , 1968, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  John Leckey,et al.  Two Hands are Better Than One , 2007 .

[43]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[44]  G. Geffen,et al.  Hemispheric equivalence for simultaneity judgements of somatosensory stimuli , 1990 .

[45]  R. Romo,et al.  Representation of moving tactile stimuli in the somatic sensory cortex of awake monkeys. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  M. Kenward,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 2007 .

[47]  J C Craig,et al.  Attending to two fingers: Two hands are better than one , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[48]  S. Nishida,et al.  Positive motion after-effect induced by bandpass-filtered random-dot kinematograms , 1992, Vision Research.

[49]  I. Birznieks,et al.  Neurophysiology of Tactile Perception : A Tribute to Steven Hsiao The tactile motion aftereffect suggests an intensive code for speed in neurons sensitive to both speed and direction of motion , 2016 .

[50]  Kenneth O. Johnson,et al.  The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[51]  E P Gardner,et al.  Objective classification of motion- and direction-sensitive neurons in primary somatosensory cortex of awake monkeys. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  M. Hollins,et al.  Perceived intensity of vibrotactile stimuli: the role of mechanoreceptive channels. , 1996, Somatosensory & motor research.

[53]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Edge-orientation processing in first-order tactile neurons , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Hanfeng Yuan,et al.  Temporal onset-order discrimination through the tactual sense: effects of frequency and site of stimulation. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[55]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness , 1996, Nature.

[56]  Susumu Tachi,et al.  Tactile motion aftereffects produced by appropriate presentation for mechanoreceptors , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  Olivier Massin,et al.  In touch. , 2002, Emergency nurse : the journal of the RCN Accident and Emergency Nursing Association.

[58]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Importance of spike timing in touch: an analogy with hearing? , 2016, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[59]  R. Romo,et al.  Frequency discrimination in the sense of flutter: psychophysical measurements correlated with postcentral events in behaving monkeys , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[60]  B L Whitsel,et al.  The capacity of human subjects to process directional information provided at two skin sites. , 1988, Somatosensory & motor research.

[61]  J. Juola,et al.  Audiovisual synchrony and temporal order judgments: Effects of experimental method and stimulus type , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[62]  Nigel H. Lovell,et al.  Convergence across Tactile Afferent Types in Primary and Secondary Somatosensory Cortices , 2014, PloS one.

[63]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Four channels mediate the mechanical aspects of touch. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[64]  E. P. Gardner,et al.  Discrimination of the direction of motion on the human hand: a psychophysical study of stimulation parameters. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  Mark Hollins,et al.  Vebrotactile adaptation of a RA system: A psychophysical analysis , 1996 .

[66]  R. Johansson,et al.  Properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the human hand related to touch sensation. , 1984, Human neurobiology.