Detecting temporal reversals in human locomotion
暂无分享,去创建一个
Francesca Figliozzi | Francesco Lacquaniti | Paolo Viviani | Giovanna Cristina Campione | P. Viviani | F. Lacquaniti | G. C. Campione | F. Figliozzi
[1] K. Verfaillie. Perceiving Human Locomotion: Priming Effects in Direction Discrimination , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[2] Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al. Global Processing of Biological Motions , 1994 .
[3] L. Maloney,et al. Planning multiple movements within a fixed time limit: the cost of constrained time allocation in a visuo-motor task. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[4] Aina Puce,et al. Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[5] D I Perrett,et al. Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.
[6] M. Pavlova,et al. Orientation specificity in biological motion perception , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[7] Francesco Lacquaniti,et al. The perception of visible speech: estimation of speech rate and detection of time reversals , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[8] R. Vogels,et al. Functional differentiation of macaque visual temporal cortical neurons using a parametric action space. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[9] Talma Hendler,et al. Neural representations of kinematic laws of motion: Evidence for action-perception coupling , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[10] D. Proffitt,et al. Judgments of natural and anomalous trajectories in the presence and absence of motion. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[11] F. Keil. The Development of the Young Child's Ability to Anticipate the Outcomes of Simple Causal Events. , 1979 .
[12] D. Perrett,et al. EPS Mid-Career Award 2008 Seeing the future : Natural image sequences produce “ anticipatory ” neuronal activity and bias perceptual report , 2009 .
[13] A. Thorstensson,et al. How is the normal locomotor program modified to produce backward walking? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] K. Nakayama,et al. Intact “biological motion” and “structure from motion” perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms: A case study , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[15] Ann L. Brown,et al. Melting chocolate and melting snowmen: Analogical reasoning and causal relations , 1990, Cognition.
[16] P. Viviani,et al. Perceiving facial expressions , 2009 .
[17] D. Perrett,et al. Integration of form and motion in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area (STPa) of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[18] E. Vatikiotis-Bateson,et al. Perceiving Biological Motion: Dissociating Visible Speech from Walking , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[19] J. F. Yang,et al. Backward walking: a simple reversal of forward walking? , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.
[20] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Human Body , 2001 .
[21] N. Birbaumer,et al. Recognition of Point-Light Biological Motion Displays by Young Children , 2001, Perception.
[22] R. Mansfield,et al. Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .
[23] R. Vogels,et al. Discrimination of locomotion direction in impoverished displays of walkers by macaque monkeys. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[24] Kiyoshi Fujimoto,et al. Motion Induction from Biological Motion , 2003, Perception.
[25] A. Saygin. Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[26] P. Viviani,et al. The effect of movement velocity on form perception: Geometric illusions in dynamic displays , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] M. Giese,et al. Visual Learning Shapes the Processing of Complex Movement Stimuli in the Human Brain , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] Roberto Cipolla,et al. Structure from motion , 2008 .
[29] J. Freyd,et al. Timing and Apparent Motion Path Choice With Human Body Photographs , 1993 .
[30] M. Farah,et al. The psychological reality of the body schema: a test with normal participants. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .
[32] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[33] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Motor patterns for human gait: backward versus forward locomotion. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] V. Stone,et al. The Body-Inversion Effect , 2003, Psychological science.
[35] Marina Pavlova,et al. Biological Motion Shown Backwards: The Apparent-Facing Effect , 2002, Perception.
[36] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Frames of reference for biological motion and face perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[37] Mukul Mukherjee,et al. Variability of lower extremity joint kinematics during backward walking in a virtual environment. , 2010, Nonlinear dynamics, psychology, and life sciences.
[38] S. Grillner. Control of Locomotion in Bipeds, Tetrapods, and Fish , 1981 .
[39] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[40] P. Haggard,et al. Experts see it all: configural effects in action observation , 2010, Psychological research.
[41] M. Giese,et al. Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception , 2006, Current Biology.
[42] Frank E. Pollick,et al. Neural Substrates for Action Understanding at Different Description Levels in the Human Brain , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[43] J. Cutting,et al. Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .
[44] James E. Cutting,et al. A program to generate synthetic walkers as dynamic point-light displays , 1978 .
[45] E S Spelke,et al. Infants' sensitivity to effects of gravity on visible object motion. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[46] P. McLeod,et al. Preserved and Impaired Detection of Structure From Motion by a 'Motion-blind" Patient , 1996 .
[47] A. Atkinson,et al. Evidence for distinct contributions of form and motion information to the recognition of emotions from body gestures , 2007, Cognition.
[48] W. Prinz,et al. Motor learning enhances perceptual judgment: a case for action-perception transfer , 2001, Psychological research.
[49] M. Pavlova,et al. Changing a natural orientation: recognition of biological motion pattern by children and adults , 1990 .
[50] P. Viviani,et al. Biological movements look uniform: evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[51] D. Heeger,et al. A Hierarchy of Temporal Receptive Windows in Human Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[52] B. Bertenthal,et al. Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[53] J. Cutting,et al. Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues , 1977 .
[54] C. Atkeson,et al. Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[55] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[56] B. Shanon. Aristotelianism, Newtonianism and the Physics of the Layman , 1976, Perception.
[57] T. Flash,et al. Neuronal encoding of human kinematic invariants during action observation. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[58] Nikolaus F Troje,et al. Reference Frames for Orientation Anisotropies in Face Recognition and Biological-Motion Perception , 2003, Perception.
[59] W. Friedman. Arrows of Time in Infancy: The Representation of Temporal–Causal Invariances , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[60] S. Sumi. Upside-down Presentation of the Johansson Moving Light-Spot Pattern , 1984, Perception.
[61] R. Blake,et al. Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.