The combined influence of binocular disparity and shading on pictorial shape

The combined influence of binocular disparity and shading on pictorial shape was studied. Stimuli were several pairs of stereo photographs of real objects. The stereo base was 0, 7, or 14 cm, and the location of the light source was varied over three positions (one from about the viewpoint of the camera, one about perpendicular to the line of sight, and one in between the two). Therefore, in total, nine different combinations were studied. Subjects had to perform surface attitude settings at about 300 positions in the image plane. From the settings, depth maps were calculated on which a principal components analysis was performed. It was found that three components were enough to account for at least 97.8% of the variance in the data. The first component accounted for shape constancy. The effects of the two cues could be isolated as a linear combination of the other two components. The effects of the disparity and the shading cue variation were found to combine in almost linear fashion.

[1]  J. Todd,et al.  Effects of Texture, Illumination, and Surface Reflectance on Stereoscopic Shape Perception , 1996, Perception.

[2]  Chris Christou,et al.  Shape Constancy in Pictorial Relief , 1996, Object Representation in Computer Vision.

[3]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Surface perception in pictures , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  V S Ramachandran,et al.  Perceiving shape from shading. , 1988, Scientific American.

[5]  M. Landy,et al.  A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues , 1993, Vision Research.

[6]  J. Todd,et al.  Effects of changing viewing conditions on the perceived structure of smoothly curved surfaces. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  John P. Frisby,et al.  Interaction of stereo, texture and outline cues in the shape perception of three-dimensional ridges , 1993, Vision Research.

[8]  Walraven jan,et al.  TNO test for stereoscopic vision , 1975 .

[9]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[10]  N. L. Johnson,et al.  Multivariate Analysis , 1958, Nature.

[11]  Eileen Kowler Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[12]  C. Blakemore Binocular depth discrimination and the nasotemporal division , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[13]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Perturbation Study of Shading in Pictures , 1996, Perception.

[14]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.

[15]  John P. Frisby,et al.  Interaction of Stereo, Texture Cues in the Shape Perception Dimensional Ridges , 1993 .

[16]  A. Parker,et al.  Integration of depth modules: Stereopsis and texture , 1993, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Depth Relief , 1995, Perception.

[18]  James T. Todd,et al.  The perception of surface orientation from multiple sources of optical information , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Integration of Depth Modules , 1990 .

[20]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Perception of local shape from shading , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  H H Bülthoff,et al.  Integration of depth modules: stereo and shading. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[22]  Andrew Blake,et al.  Surface descriptions from stereo and shading , 1986, Image Vis. Comput..

[23]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Psychophysical estimation of the human depth combination rule , 1991, Other Conferences.

[24]  H Collewijn,et al.  Binocular eye movements and the perception of depth. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[25]  Allen Brookes,et al.  Integrating stereopsis with monocular interpretations of planar surfaces , 1988, Vision Research.

[26]  F. A. Miles Binocular Vision and Stereopsis by Ian P. Howard and Brian J. Rogers, Oxford University Press, 1995. £90.00 (736 pages) ISBN 0 19 508476 4. , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.