Exaggerated Versus Representative Exemplification in News Reports

A news report on carjacking, presented in magazine format, was manipulated to create versions differing in exemplar distortion (minimally, mildly, substantially, and extremely) and precision of base rate information (imprecise, precise). Time of assessment (immediate, delayed) was varied as well. Readers evaluated carjacking as a national issue, as likely to worsen, as a local danger, and as a personal threat. They also estimated the proportion of carjacking victims suffering injury. The readers eventually rated aspects of the news report. Readers of news featuring extreme exemplar distortion considered carjacking to be a more serious national problem than did readers of news featuring minimally, mildly, or substantially distorted exemplars. This effect was uniform for precision of base rate information and was stable over time. Readers presented with exemplars of people killed during carjackings grossly overestimated the incidence of such an outcome. Additionally, they found the report more upsetting than did readers presented with less extreme exemplars. There were no significant differences, however, in evaluations of newsworthiness, importance, or accuracy of the various versions of the report.

[1]  Hans-Bernd Brosius,et al.  The Utility of Exemplars in Persuasive Communications , 1994 .

[2]  Dolf Zillmann,et al.  The Impact of Quotation in News Reports on Issue Perception , 1993 .

[3]  P. Tannenbaum The Entertainment Functions of Television , 1980 .

[4]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  When Do Base Rates Affect Predictions , 1981 .

[5]  H. Markus Self-schemata and processing information about the self. , 1977 .

[6]  S. Shyam Sundar,et al.  Impression-formation effects of printed news varying in descriptive precision and exemplifications. , 1992 .

[7]  L. Perloff,et al.  Perceptions of Vulnerability to Victimization , 1983 .

[8]  A. Tversky,et al.  Affect, Generalization, and the Perception of Risk. , 1983 .

[9]  J. David Kennamer,et al.  News Values and the Vividness of Information , 1988 .

[10]  N D Weinstein,et al.  Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility. , 1984, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[11]  J. B. Haskins The Trouble with Bad News , 1981 .

[12]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[13]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[14]  Philip E. Meyer,et al.  The New Precision Journalism , 1991 .

[15]  Shanto Iyengar,et al.  THE ACCESSIBILITY BIAS IN POLITICS: TELEVISION NEWS AND PUBLIC OPINION , 1990 .

[16]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[17]  L. Perloff,et al.  Self–other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. , 1986 .

[18]  G. Bower,et al.  Judgmental biases resulting from differing availabilities of arguments. , 1980 .

[19]  Nancy E. Avis,et al.  Base rates can affect individual predictions. , 1980 .

[20]  A. Paivio Imagery and verbal processes , 1972 .

[21]  A. Tversky,et al.  Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness , 1972 .

[22]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .

[23]  J. Bryant,et al.  Responding to the screen : reception and reaction processes , 1991 .

[24]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty , 1982 .

[25]  D. Zillmann,et al.  Effects of upbeat stories in broadcast news , 1994 .

[26]  John E. Newhagen,et al.  The Evening's Bad News: Effects of Compelling Negative Television News Images on Memory , 1992 .

[27]  Teresa M. Amabile,et al.  Social Roles, Social Control and Biases in Social Perception , 1977 .

[28]  S. Iyengar Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. , 1991 .

[29]  S. Fulero,et al.  From individual to group impressions: Availability heuristics in stereotype formation , 1978 .