Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds.

Recent studies on nonsymbolic arithmetic have illustrated that under conditions that prevent exact calculation, adults display a systematic tendency to overestimate the answers to addition problems and underestimate the answers to subtraction problems. It has been suggested that this operational momentum results from exposure to a culture-specific practice of representing numbers spatially; alternatively, the mind may represent numbers in spatial terms from early in development. In the current study, we asked whether operational momentum is present during infancy, prior to exposure to culture-specific representations of numbers. Infants (9-month-olds) were shown videos of events involving the addition or subtraction of objects with three different types of outcomes: numerically correct, too large, and too small. Infants looked significantly longer only at those incorrect outcomes that violated the momentum of the arithmetic operation (i.e., at too-large outcomes in subtraction events and too-small outcomes in addition events). The presence of operational momentum during infancy indicates developmental continuity in the underlying mechanisms used when operating over numerical representations.

[1]  S. Carey,et al.  The Representations Underlying Infants' Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes , 2002, Psychological science.

[2]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2007, Developmental science.

[3]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy , 2002, Cognition.

[4]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Amount Versus Number: Infants' Use of Area and Contour Length to Discriminate Small Sets , 2001 .

[5]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Numerical Transformations in Five-month-old Human Infants. , 1997 .

[6]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[7]  Koleen McCrink,et al.  Ratio Abstraction by 6-Month-Old Infants , 2007, Psychological science.

[8]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' Discrimination of Number vs. Continuous Extent , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal arithmetic in humans: Light from noise , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  John R. Platt,et al.  Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: Effects of error contingencies , 1971 .

[11]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[12]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  When the ordinary seems unexpected: evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants , 2005, Cognition.

[13]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[14]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .

[15]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants , 2008, PLoS biology.

[16]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[17]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[18]  Fei Xu,et al.  Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations , 2003, Cognition.

[19]  B. Rourke,et al.  Arithmetic Disabilities, Specific and Otherwise , 1993, Journal of learning disabilities.

[20]  K. Hiraki,et al.  Baby arithmetic: one object plus one tone , 2004, Cognition.

[21]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Children’s understanding of the relationship between addition and subtraction , 2008, Cognition.

[22]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[23]  K. Wynn,et al.  Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.

[24]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[25]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[26]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic , 2008, Cognition.

[27]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children , 2006, Cognition.

[28]  T. Hubbard Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[29]  M. Casasola,et al.  Six-month-old infants' categorization of containment spatial relations. , 2003, Child development.

[30]  D. Geary Mathematical disabilities: cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[32]  Melissa W. Clearfield,et al.  Familiarization in Infants' Perception of Addition Problems , 2006 .