Phantom flashes caused by interactions across visual space.

Studies regarding the effects of context on the perception of a visual target's temporal properties have generally addressed the cross-modal integration of auditory context, within a functional or ecological (e.g., Bayesian) framework. A deeper understanding of contextual effects in temporal vision may be gained by drawing connections with the rich models of signal processing developed in the field of spatial vision. To bridge this gap, we investigate a purely visual version of the cross-modal "double-flash" illusion (L. Shams, Y. Kamitani, & S. Shimojo, 2000; J. T. Wilson & W. Singer, 1981). Here, a single target flash can be perceived as several flashes if it is presented in the context of multiple visual inducers. This effect is robust across conditions where the target and inducers are of opposite contrast polarity, in different hemifields, are non-collinear, are presented dichoptically, or are high-frequency Gabor patches. The effect diminishes when target-inducer distance is increased or when the target is moved toward the fovea. When the target is foveated, the effect can still be recovered if the inducers are placed at 3° distance. Finally, we find that multiple target flashes are not "merged" into a smaller number of perceived flashes when presented with singular inducers. These results suggest a cortical mechanism based on isotropic propagation of transient signals or possibly based on higher level event detection. Finally, we find that multiple target flashes are not "merged" into a smaller number of perceived flashes when presented with singular inducers. These results suggest a mechanism based on the propagation of transient signals and argue against the relevance of the cue integration model developed for the cross-modal version of the effect.

[1]  M. McCourt,et al.  Nearly instantaneous brightness induction. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[2]  Uri Polat,et al.  The relationship between the subjective and objective aspects of visual filling-in , 2007, Vision Research.

[3]  Haluk Öğmen,et al.  Meta- and paracontrast reveal differences between contour- and brightness-processing mechanisms , 2006, Vision Research.

[4]  Ulrik R Beierholm,et al.  Sound-induced flash illusion as an optimal percept , 2005, Neuroreport.

[5]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[6]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Signal Strength Determines the Nature of the Relationship Between Perception and Working Memory , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Circuits for Local and Global Signal Integration in Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  E. John The neurophysics of consciousness , 2002, Brain Research Reviews.

[9]  E. G. Jones,et al.  The thalamic matrix and thalamocortical synchrony , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[10]  J. Budd,et al.  Local lateral connectivity of inhibitory clutch cells in layer 4 of cat visual cortex (area 17) , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Compression of space in visual memory , 2001, Vision Research.

[12]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Illusions: What you see is what you hear , 2000, Nature.

[13]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  S. Shimojo,et al.  In Space, the past Can Be Recast but Not the Present , 2000, Perception.

[15]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Interactions between attention, context and learning in primary visual cortex , 2000, Vision Research.

[16]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Attention Modulates Contextual Influences in the Primary Visual Cortex of Alert Monkeys , 1999, Neuron.

[17]  J. Fadiman Illusions , 1999 .

[18]  R. Llinás,et al.  The neuronal basis for consciousness. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[19]  Idan Segev,et al.  Ion Channel Stochasticity May Be Critical in Determining the Reliability and Precision of Spike Timing , 1998, Neural Computation.

[20]  R. Leigh,et al.  Saccades to remembered targets: The effects of saccades and illusory stimulus motion , 1998, Vision Research.

[21]  E. Todorov,et al.  A local circuit approach to understanding integration of long-range inputs in primary visual cortex. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  U. Polat,et al.  Collinear stimuli regulate visual responses depending on cell's contrast threshold , 1998, Nature.

[23]  Talis Bachmann,et al.  Visibility of Brief Images: The Dual-Process Approach , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[24]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Characteristics of surround inhibition in cat area 17 , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  UTE LEONARDS,et al.  Selective Temporal Interactions Between Processing Streams with Differential Sensitivity for Colour and Luminance Contrast , 1997, Vision Research.

[26]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Contrast dependence of contextual effects in primate visual cortex , 1997, nature.

[27]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Relation between patterns of intrinsic lateral connectivity, ocular dominance, and cytochrome oxidase-reactive regions in macaque monkey striate cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  M Stemmler,et al.  Lateral interactions in primary visual cortex: a model bridging physiology and psychophysics. , 1995, Science.

[29]  V. Manahilov,et al.  Spatiotemporal visual response to suprathreshold stimuli , 1995, Vision Research.

[30]  U Polat,et al.  Spatial interactions in human vision: from near to far via experience-dependent cascades of connections. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  C D Gilbert,et al.  Early perceptual learning. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  U. Polat,et al.  The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions , 1994, Vision Research.

[33]  Louis J. DeFelice,et al.  Limitations of the Hodgkin-Huxley Formalism: Effects of Single Channel Kinetics on Transmembrane Voltage Dynamics , 1993, Neural Computation.

[34]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Motion capture changes to induced motion at higher luminance contrasts, smaller eccentricities, and larger inducer sizes , 1993, Vision Research.

[35]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[36]  R. Llinás,et al.  Coherent 40-Hz oscillation characterizes dream state in humans. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  P. Wenderoth,et al.  The different mechanisms of the direct and indirect tilt illusions , 1988, Vision Research.

[38]  Talis Bachmann,et al.  Time course of the subjective contrast enhancement for a second stimulus in successively paired above-threshold transient forms: Perceptual retouch instead of forward masking , 1988, Vision Research.

[39]  Mark A. Georgeson,et al.  Temporal properties of spatial contrast vision , 1987, Vision Research.

[40]  J. Wilson Interaction of Simultaneous Visual Events , 1987, Perception.

[41]  D. Stork,et al.  Temporal impulse responses from flicker sensitivities. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[42]  Mitsuo Ikeda,et al.  Temporal impulse response , 1986, Vision Research.

[43]  Michael A. Webster,et al.  Temporal properties of brightness and color induction , 1986, Vision Research.

[44]  Robert B. Welch,et al.  Contributions of audition and vision to temporal rate perception , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  S P McKee,et al.  Discrimination of time: comparison of foveal and peripheral sensitivity. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[46]  Peter W. Schiller,et al.  Opiate receptor subclasses differ in their conformational requirements , 1982, Nature.

[47]  J. Wilson,et al.  Visual persistence at both onset and offset of stimulation , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[48]  W Singer,et al.  Simultaneous visual events show a long-range spatial interaction , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  H. Wilson Spatiotemporal characterization of a transient mechanism in the human visual system , 1980, Vision Research.

[50]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[51]  P. Schiller,et al.  Composition of geniculostriate input ot superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  P. Schiller,et al.  Functional specificity of lateral geniculate nucleus laminae of the rhesus monkey. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  H Spekreijse,et al.  Auditory—Visual Interactions and the Correspondence between Perceived Auditory Space and Perceived Visual Space , 1977, Perception.

[54]  P. Schiller,et al.  Properties and tectal projections of monkey retinal ganglion cells. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  R. Barth,et al.  The shift-effect in retinal ganglion cells of the rhesus monkey , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[57]  D. Jameson,et al.  Mach bands : quantitative studies on neural networks in the retina , 1966 .

[58]  T SHIPLEY,et al.  Auditory Flutter-Driving of Visual Flicker , 1964, Science.

[59]  Mowbray Gh,et al.  On discriminating the rate of visual flicker and auditory flutter. , 1959 .

[60]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  J. Mcilwain Some evidence concerning the physiological basis of the periphery effect in the cat's retina , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[62]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[63]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[64]  T. Bachmann The process of perceptual retouch: Nonspecific afferent activation dynamics in explaining visual masking , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[65]  C. Gilbert Microcircuitry of the visual cortex. , 1983, Annual review of neuroscience.

[66]  A. K. Myers,et al.  Matching the rate of concurrent tone bursts and light flashes as a function of flash surround luminance , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[67]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[68]  G H MOWBRAY,et al.  On discriminating the rate of visual flicker and auditory flutter. , 1959, The American journal of psychology.

[69]  Hermann Lotze,et al.  Grundzüge der psychologie , 1912 .

[70]  A. H. Lloyd Grundz?ge der psychologie. , 1901 .

[71]  F. Zöllner Ueber eine neue Art von Pseudoskopie und ihre Beziehungen zu den von Plateau und Oppel beschriebenen Bewegungsphänomenen , 1860 .