Neural correlates of implied motion

Current views of the visual system assume that the primate brain analyses form and motion along largely independent pathways; they provide no insight into why form is sometimes interpreted as motion. In a series of psychophysical and electrophysiological experiments in humans and macaques, here we show that some form information is processed in the prototypical motion areas of the superior temporal sulcus (STS). First, we show that STS cells respond to dynamic Glass patterns, which contain no coherent motion but suggest a path of motion. Second, we show that when motion signals conflict with form signals suggesting a different path of motion, both humans and monkeys perceive motion in a compromised direction. This compromise also has a correlate in the responses of STS cells, which alter their direction preferences in the presence of conflicting implied motion information. We conclude that cells in the prototypical motion areas in the dorsal visual cortex process form that implies motion. Estimating motion by combining motion cues with form cues may be a strategy to deal with the complexities of motion perception in our natural environment.

[1]  D Margoliash,et al.  Preference for autogenous song by auditory neurons in a song system nucleus of the white-crowned sparrow , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  E. Miller,et al.  Effects of Visual Experience on the Representation of Objects in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2000, Neuron.

[3]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. , 1995, Science.

[4]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  G. E. Vates,et al.  Auditory pathways of caudal telencephalon and their relation to the song system of adult male zebra finches (Taenopygia guttata) , 1996, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[6]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.

[7]  S. H. Hulse,et al.  Perceptual mechanisms for individual vocal recognition in European starlings,Sturnus vulgaris , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  L. Glass Moiré Effect from Random Dots , 1969, Nature.

[9]  M. Kilgard,et al.  Plasticity of temporal information processing in the primary auditory cortex , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.

[11]  C. Müller,et al.  Feature extraction and tonotopic organization in the avian auditory forebrain , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.

[13]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Effects of shape-discrimination training on the selectivity of inferotemporal cells in adult monkeys. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  R A Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. III. Modeling , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[16]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey. , 2001, Visual neuroscience.

[17]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Signals in Macaque Striate Cortical Neurons that Support the Perception of Glass Patterns , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Differential representation of species-specific primate vocalizations in the auditory cortices of marmoset and cat. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  H. Leppelsack,et al.  Responses of auditory neurons in the forebrain of a songbird to stimulation with species-specific sounds , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[21]  K. Sen,et al.  Feature analysis of natural sounds in the songbird auditory forebrain. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  Dynamics of the Vocal Imitation Process: How a Zebra Finch Learns Its Song , 2001, Science.

[23]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Eye position effects in monkey cortex. I. Visual and pursuit-related activity in extrastriate areas MT and MST. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  H. Wilson,et al.  Concentric orientation summation in human form vision , 1997, Vision Research.

[25]  T. Pasternak,et al.  Transient and permanent deficits in motion perception after lesions of cortical areas MT and MST in the macaque monkey. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Earl K. Miller,et al.  Selective representation of relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[29]  John Ross,et al.  Direct Evidence That “Speedlines” Influence Motion Mechanisms , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys , 1995, Current Biology.

[31]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  Norman M. Weinberger,et al.  Classical conditioning induces CS-specific receptive field plasticity in the auditory cortex of the guinea pig , 1990, Brain Research.

[33]  Michael P. Kilgard,et al.  Order-sensitive plasticity in adult primary auditory cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Continuous mapping of direction selectivity in the cat's visual cortex , 1976, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  C. Gilbert,et al.  The Neural Basis of Perceptual Learning , 2001, Neuron.

[36]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[38]  D. Margoliash Acoustic parameters underlying the responses of song-specific neurons in the white-crowned sparrow , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[39]  Gilles Laurent,et al.  Odor- and context-dependent modulation of mitral cell activity in behaving rats , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  D Jancke,et al.  Orientation Formed by a Spot's Trajectory: A Two-Dimensional Population Approach in Primary Visual Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Activation in Human MT/MST by Static Images with Implied Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.