The genetic architecture of pediatric cognitive abilities in the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort

The objective of this analysis was to examine the genetic architecture of diverse cognitive abilities in children and adolescents, including the magnitude of common genetic effects and patterns of shared and unique genetic influences. Subjects included 3689 members of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort, a general population sample comprising those aged 8–21 years who completed an extensive battery of cognitive tests. We used genome-wide complex trait analysis to estimate the SNP-based heritability of each domain, as well as the genetic correlation between all domains that showed significant genetic influence. Several of the individual domains suggested strong influence of common genetic variants (for example, reading ability, h2g=0.43, P=4e−06; emotion identification, h2g=0.36, P=1e−05; verbal memory, h2g=0.24, P=0.005). The genetic correlations highlighted trait domains that are candidates for joint interrogation in future genetic studies (for example, language reasoning and spatial reasoning, r(g)=0.72, P=0.007). These results can be used to structure future genetic and neuropsychiatric investigations of diverse cognitive abilities.

[1]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Common DNA Markers Can Account for More Than Half of the Genetic Influence on Cognitive Abilities , 2013, Psychological science.

[2]  D. Boomsma,et al.  Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to 12. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[3]  Billy L. Smith,et al.  Relationship between the Wide Range Achievement Test 3 and the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test , 1998, Psychological reports.

[4]  C. Reynolds,et al.  Behavior Genetics of Cognition Across the Lifespan , 2014 .

[5]  P. Visscher,et al.  DNA Evidence for Strong Genome-Wide Pleiotropy of Cognitive and Learning Abilities , 2013, Behavior genetics.

[6]  Evan T. Geller,et al.  Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders , 2012, Nature.

[7]  Patrick F. Sullivan,et al.  Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: the emerging picture and its implications , 2012, Nature Reviews Genetics.

[8]  Manuel A. R. Ferreira,et al.  PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. , 2007, American journal of human genetics.

[9]  Ian J. Deary,et al.  Genetic contributions to stability and change in intelligence from childhood to old age , 2012, Nature.

[10]  Raquel E Gur,et al.  Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21. , 2012, Neuropsychology.

[11]  John Blangero,et al.  Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[12]  P. Visscher,et al.  Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L , 2014, Molecular Psychiatry.

[13]  D. Boomsma,et al.  Heritability of Selective Attention and Working Memory in Preschoolers , 2005, Behavior genetics.

[14]  J. Defries,et al.  Cognitive Abilities in Childhood and Adolescence , 2014 .

[15]  Stephan Ripke,et al.  Estimating the proportion of variation in susceptibility to schizophrenia captured by common SNPs , 2012, Nature Genetics.

[16]  Michael F. Green,et al.  Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. , 2007, Archives of general psychiatry.

[17]  Jianxin Shi,et al.  Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs , 2013, Nature Genetics.

[18]  N. Martin,et al.  The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood , 2010, Molecular Psychiatry.

[19]  W. G. Hill,et al.  Genome partitioning of genetic variation for complex traits using common SNPs , 2011, Nature Genetics.

[20]  Lorna M. Lopez,et al.  Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic , 2011, Molecular Psychiatry.

[21]  P. Visscher,et al.  GCTA: a tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. , 2011, American journal of human genetics.

[22]  Anders D. Børglum,et al.  Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci , 2011, Nature Genetics.

[23]  D. Boomsma,et al.  Longitudinal genetic study of verbal and nonverbal IQ from early childhood to young adulthood , 2007 .

[24]  R. Gur,et al.  A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: Standardization and initial construct validation , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[25]  M. Daly,et al.  Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis , 2013, The Lancet.

[26]  C. Gillberg,et al.  Autism spectrum disorders and autistic like traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. , 2012, Archives of general psychiatry.

[27]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Generalist genes: implications for the cognitive sciences , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  R. Plomin,et al.  Specific cognitive abilities in 5- to 12-year-old twins , 1980, Behavior genetics.

[29]  R. Plomin,et al.  Intelligence indexes generalist genes for cognitive abilities , 2013, Intelligence.