Who should be called to the lab? A comprehensive comparison of students and non-students in classic experimental games

This study compares the behavior of students and non-students in a number of classic experimental games. We find that students are more likely to behave as homo-economicus agents than non-students in games involving other-regarding preferences (Dictator Game, Trust Game and Public Good Game). These differences persist even when controlling for demographics, cognitive ability and risk preferences. In games that do not engage other-regarding preferences (Beauty-contest and Second-price Auction) there is limited evidence of differences in behaviour between subject pools. In none of the five games is there evidence of significant differences in comprehension between students and non-students. Within subject analyses indicate that students are highly consistent in their other-regarding preferences while non-student subjects are inconsistent across other-regarding games. Our findings suggest that experiments using students will provide a lower bound estimate of other-regardedness in the general population while exaggerating the stability of other-regarding preferences.

[1]  John C. Ham,et al.  Selection Bias, Demographic Effects and Ability Effects in Common Value Auction Experiments , 2005 .

[2]  Charles A. Holt,et al.  Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects , 2002 .

[3]  Steven D. Levitt,et al.  What Do Laboratory Experiments Measuring Social Preferences Reveal About the Real World , 2007 .

[4]  James Andreoni,et al.  Why free ride?: Strategies and learning in public goods experiments , 1988 .

[5]  A. Rustichini,et al.  Self Selection Does Not Increase Other-Regarding Preferences Among Adult Laboratory Subjects, But Student Subjects May Be More Self-Regarding than Adults , 2010, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[6]  R. Nagel Unraveling in Guessing Games: An Experimental Study , 1995 .

[7]  J. Weinstein,et al.  Coethnicity: Diversity and the Dilemmas of Collective Action , 2009 .

[8]  Alexander W. Cappelen,et al.  Efficiency, Equality and Reciprocity in Social Preferences: A Comparison of Students and a Representative Population. , 2010 .

[9]  Alexander W. Cappelen,et al.  Social Preferences in the Lab: A Comparison of Students and a Representative Population , 2011, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[10]  V. Smith,et al.  Preferences, Property Rights, and Anonymity in Bargaining Games , 1994 .

[11]  S. Yen,et al.  An econometric analysis of household donations in the USA , 2002 .

[12]  W. Güth,et al.  An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining , 1982 .

[13]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Sophisticated EWA Learning and Strategic Teaching in Repeated Games , 2000 .

[14]  Rachel T. A. Croson,et al.  Gender Differences in Preferences , 2009 .

[15]  J. Carpenter,et al.  Altruistic behavior in a representative dictator experiment , 2008 .

[16]  A. Danielson,et al.  Do You Trust Your Brethren? Eliciting Trust Attitudes and Trust Behavior in a Tanzanian Congregation , 2007 .

[17]  Catherine C. Eckel,et al.  Is Trust a Risky Decision? , 2004 .

[18]  Timothy N. Cason,et al.  The strategy method lowers measured trustworthy behavior , 2009 .

[19]  V. Smith,et al.  Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games , 2000 .

[20]  John M. Fitzgerald,et al.  Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History: A Re-examination , 2000 .

[21]  A. Riedl,et al.  The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  George Gamow,et al.  One, Two, Three...Infinity: Facts and Speculations of Science , 1947 .

[23]  Rosemarie Nagel,et al.  One, Two, (Three), Infinity: Newspaper and Lab Beauty-Contest Experiments , 2000 .

[24]  Aldo Rustichini,et al.  Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Steven D. Levitt,et al.  Viewpoint: On the Generalizability of Lab Behaviour to the Field , 2007 .

[26]  J. Andreoni Cooperation in Public-Goods Experiments: Kindness or Confusion? , 1995 .

[27]  R. Peterson On the Use of College Students in Social Science Research: Insights from a Second‐Order Meta‐analysis , 2001 .

[28]  E. Fehr,et al.  Does Fairness Prevent Market Clearing? An Experimental Investigation , 1993 .

[29]  Christian Thöni,et al.  Trust, voluntary cooperation, and socio-economic background: survey and experimental evidence , 2004 .

[30]  J. Kagel,et al.  Handbook of Experimental Economics , 1997 .

[31]  Matthias Sutter,et al.  Trust and trustworthiness across different age groups , 2007, Games Econ. Behav..

[32]  J. Carpenter,et al.  Comparing Students to Workers: The Effects of Social Framing on Behavior in Distribution Games , 2004, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[33]  R. Aumann,et al.  Unraveling in Guessing Games : An Experimental Study , 2007 .

[34]  Joyce E. Berg,et al.  Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History , 1995 .

[35]  Joseph Hilbe,et al.  Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models , 2009 .

[36]  Catherine C. Eckel,et al.  Altruism in Anonymous Dictator Games , 1996 .

[37]  Jordi Brandts,et al.  How Universal is Behavior? A Four Country Comparison of Spite and Cooperation in Voluntary Contribution Mechanisms , 2003 .

[38]  Dinah Pura T. Depositario,et al.  Should students be used as subjects in experimental auctions , 2009 .

[39]  Charles R. Plott,et al.  The control of game form recognition in experiments: understanding dominant strategy failures in a simple two person “guessing” game , 2009 .

[40]  R. Duch,et al.  It's Not Whether You Win or Lose, but How You Play the Game: Self-Interest, Social Justice, and Mass Attitudes toward Market Transition , 2004, American Political Science Review.

[41]  H. Gintis,et al.  The evolution of strong reciprocity: cooperation in heterogeneous populations. , 2004, Theoretical population biology.

[42]  O. Volij,et al.  Field Centipedes , 2006 .

[43]  D. Rubin Matched Sampling for Causal Effects: Matching to Remove Bias in Observational Studies , 1973 .

[44]  Hans-Theo Normann,et al.  A Within-Subject Analysis of Other-Regarding Preferences , 2010, Games Econ. Behav..

[45]  H. Gintis,et al.  The Evolution of Strong Reciprocity , 2000 .

[46]  J. Kagel,et al.  Independent Private Value Auctions: Bidder Behaviour in First-, Second- and Third-Price Auctions with Varying Numbers of Bidders , 1993 .

[47]  Chetan Dave,et al.  Eliciting risk preferences: When is simple better? , 2008 .

[48]  Colin Camerer Progress in Behavioral Game Theory , 1997 .

[49]  U. Fischbacher z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments , 1999 .

[50]  E. Fehr A Theory of Fairness, Competition and Cooperation , 1998 .

[51]  James Andreoni,et al.  Giving according to GARP: an experimental test of the rationality of altruism , 1999 .

[52]  R. Mark Isaac,et al.  Group Size Effects in Public Goods Provision: The Voluntary Contributions Mechanism , 1988 .

[53]  V. Smith,et al.  Social Distance and Other-Regarding Behavior in Dictator Games: Reply , 1999 .

[54]  Donald B. Rubin,et al.  Bayesian Inference for Causal Effects: The Role of Randomization , 1978 .

[55]  Petra E. Todd,et al.  Matching As An Econometric Evaluation Estimator: Evidence from Evaluating a Job Training Programme , 1997 .