Ascribed Status, Lexical Diversity, and Accent: Determinants of Perceived Status, Solidarity, and Control of Speech Style

The effects of status ascriptions (high vs. low) and accent (standard American vs. Mexican-American) upon perceptions of speaker status and solidarity are examined. Consequences of the independent variables for a novel dependent variable, perceived control of speech style, are examined also. One hundred and twenty-two respondents rated one of eight message versions in a between-group design. Results indicated, among other things, that: (1) status ascriptions and accent combined additively to affect judgements of speaker status, as did status ascriptions and diversity; (2) lexical diversity and accent affected both solidarity judgements and status judgements interactively, although the nature of the interactive effect differed across measures; and (3) lexical diversity was an especially potent determinant of perceptions of control of speech style. It is suggested that a clear picture of linguistic and non-linguistic determinants of judgements of speaker status is emerging.

[1]  J. Rotter Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. , 1966, Psychological monographs.

[2]  W. Lambert A Social Psychology of Bilingualism , 1967 .

[3]  Donald M. Taylor,et al.  Evaluational Reactions to Discrepancies Between Social Class and Language , 1974 .

[4]  H. Giles,et al.  Speech style and social evaluation , 1975 .

[5]  James J. Bradac,et al.  Two studies of the effects of linguistic diversity upon judgments of communicator attributes and message effectiveness , 1976 .

[6]  A. Mulac Assessment and application of the revised speech dialect attitudinal scale , 1976 .

[7]  Greg W. Schmidt,et al.  The effects of perceived status and linguistic diversity upon judgment of speaker attributes and message effectiveness. , 1976, The Journal of psychology.

[8]  James J. Bradac,et al.  Diversity and density: Lexically determined evaluative and informational consequences of linguistic complexity , 1977 .

[9]  G. S. Stern,et al.  Interal locus of control as a value , 1977 .

[10]  Lawrence A. Hosman,et al.  Reciprocal disclosures and language intensity: Attributional consequences , 1978 .

[11]  M. Snyder Self-monitoring processes , 1979 .

[12]  H. Giles,et al.  Some behavioural consequences of speech and dress styles , 1979 .

[13]  R. Sebastian,et al.  The effects of speech style and social class background on social judgements of speakers , 1980 .

[14]  B. L. Brown Effects of Speech Rate on Personality Attributions and Competency Evaluations , 1980 .

[15]  K. Scherer,et al.  Social Markers in Speech , 1980 .

[16]  J. Bradac,et al.  On the neglected side of linguistic science: multivariate studies of sentence judgment , 1980 .

[17]  A. Mulac,et al.  Differences in perceptions created by syntactic‐semantic productions of male and female speakers , 1980 .

[18]  James J. Bradac,et al.  Effects of Intensity, Immediacy and Diversity Upon Receiver Attitudes Toward a Belief-Discrepant Message and Its Source , 1980 .

[19]  James J. Bradac,et al.  Language style on trial: Effects of “powerful” and “powerless” speech upon judgments of victims and villains , 1981 .

[20]  N. Anderson Foundations of information integration theory , 1981 .

[21]  Anthony Conway,et al.  Accent and lexical diversity as determinants of impression formation and perceived employment suitability , 1981 .

[22]  Robert N. St. Clair,et al.  Language and social psychology , 1981 .

[23]  C. Bulik,et al.  Evaluations of Middle Class and Lower Class Speakers of Standard American and German-Accented English , 1982 .

[24]  R. Day Attitudes Toward Language Variation: Social and Applied Contexts , 1982 .

[25]  H. Giles,et al.  The effect of speaker's accent, social class background and message style on British listeners' social judgements , 1983 .

[26]  A. Mulac,et al.  Attributional Consequences of Powerful and Powerless Speech Styles in a Crisis-Intervention Context , 1984 .