Cognitive changes in conjunctive rule-based category learning: An ERP approach
暂无分享,去创建一个
Rahel Rabi | John Paul Minda | Tianshu Zhu | Marc F. Joanisse | J. P. Minda | M. Joanisse | Rahel Rabi | Tianshu Zhu
[1] F. Gregory Ashby,et al. Multidimensional Models of Perception and Cognition , 2014 .
[2] R. Poldrack,et al. Category learning and the memory systems debate , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[3] M. Riesenhuber,et al. Categorization Training Results in Shape- and Category-Selective Human Neural Plasticity , 2007, Neuron.
[4] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[5] Michael D. Rugg,et al. Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory , 1998, Nature.
[6] W. T. Maddox,et al. Cortical and subcortical brain regions involved in rule-based category learning , 2005, Neuroreport.
[7] Julia Herschensohn,et al. Age-related effects , 2013 .
[8] F. Ashby,et al. The effects of concurrent task interference on category learning: Evidence for multiple category learning systems , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[9] Christopher Canty. Task switching in people with Parkinson's disease: the role of the basal ganglia , 2012 .
[10] Jonathan R. Folstein,et al. After the P3: late executive processes in stimulus categorization. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[11] W Todd Maddox,et al. Development of implicit and explicit category learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[12] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Heterarchical reinforcement-learning model for integration of multiple cortico-striatal loops: fMRI examination in stimulus-action-reward association learning , 2006, Neural Networks.
[13] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[14] M. Petrides,et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] J. P. Minda,et al. Familiarization May Minimize Age-Related Declines in Rule-Based Category Learning , 2017, Psychology and aging.
[16] W. Todd Maddox,et al. Rule-based and information-integration category learning in normal aging , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[17] R. Cameron Craddock,et al. Predicting intrinsic brain activity , 2013, NeuroImage.
[18] W Todd Maddox,et al. Removing the Frontal Lobes , 2010, Psychological science.
[19] J. P. Minda,et al. Category learning in older adulthood: A study of the Shepard, Hovland, and Jenkins (1961) tasks. , 2016, Psychology and aging.
[20] P. J. Reber,et al. A review of medial temporal lobe and caudate contributions to visual category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[21] R. Romo,et al. Functional properties of primate putamen neurons during the categorization of tactile stimuli. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] Carol A. Seger,et al. Category learning in the brain. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.
[23] R. Morrison,et al. Dissociation of category-learning systems via brain potentials , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[24] Rahel Rabi,et al. Rule-Based Category Learning in Children: The Role of Age and Executive Functioning , 2014, PloS one.
[25] Daniel S. Ruchkin,et al. 11 Positive Slow Wave and P300: Association and Disassociation , 1983 .
[26] A. Kok. Effects of degradation of visual stimuli on components of the event-related potential (ERP) in go/nogo reaction tasks , 1986, Biological Psychology.
[27] J. P. Minda,et al. Ego depletion interferes with rule-defined category learning but not non-rule-defined category learning , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[28] F. Ashby,et al. Category learning and multiple memory systems , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[29] John Paul Minda,et al. Learning rule-described and non-rule-described categories: a comparison of children and adults. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[30] Haline E. Schendan,et al. Object-sensitive activity reflects earlier perceptual and later cognitive processing of visual objects between 95 and 500ms , 2010, Brain Research.
[31] W T Maddox,et al. Comparing decision bound and exemplar models of categorization , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[32] Edward E. Smith,et al. Alternative strategies of categorization , 1998, Cognition.
[33] M. Bradley,et al. Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[34] Jacqueline A. Rushby,et al. Event-related potential correlates of phasic and tonic measures of the orienting reflex , 2007, Biological Psychology.
[35] Sarah J. Miles,et al. The effects of concurrent verbal and visual tasks on category learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[36] M. L. Kietzman,et al. Slow wave and P300 in signal detection. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[37] Murray Grossman,et al. Multiple systems of category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[38] A Kok,et al. Effects of visual field, stimulus degradation, and level of practice on event-related potentials of the brain. , 1985, Psychophysiology.
[39] Rahel Rabi,et al. Learning categories via rules and similarity: comparing adults and children. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[40] A. Kok,et al. Components of the event-related potential following degraded and undegraded visual stimuli , 1980, Biological Psychology.
[41] Murray Grossman,et al. The Neural Basis for Category-Specific Knowledge: An fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.
[42] D. Ruchkin,et al. Toward a functional categorization of slow waves. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[43] Emanuel Donchin,et al. The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.
[44] Carol A. Seger,et al. Dissociation between Striatal Regions while Learning to Categorize via Feedback and via Observation , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] Ray Johnson. P300: A Model of the Variables Controlling Its Amplitude a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[46] E. Donchin,et al. Parsing the late positive complex: mental chronometry and the ERP components that inhabit the neighborhood of the P300. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[47] Stephen D. Mayhew,et al. Article Learning Shapes the Representation of Behavioral Choice in the Human Brain , 2022 .
[48] Shawn W. Ell,et al. Prefrontal Contributions to Rule-based and Information-integration Category Learning , 2022 .
[49] Age-related effects of different types of noise and stimulus quality: an event-related potential (ERP) study , 1996 .
[50] W. T. Maddox,et al. Dual-task interference in perceptual category learning , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[51] Ziv M. Williams,et al. Selective enhancement of associative learning by microstimulation of the anterior caudate , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[52] Carol A. Seger,et al. How do the basal ganglia contribute to categorization? Their roles in generalization, response selection, and learning via feedback , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[53] J. P. Minda,et al. Better Mood and Better Performance , 2010, Psychological science.
[54] G. Mulder,et al. Language, memory, and aging: an electrophysiological exploration of the N400 during reading of memory-demanding sentences. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[55] Steven J. Luck,et al. ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[56] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[57] F. Gregory Ashby,et al. THE ROLE OF THE BASAL GANGLIA IN CATEGORY LEARNING , 2006 .
[58] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. , 2005, Psychophysiology.