Cognitive changes in conjunctive rule-based category learning: An ERP approach

When learning rule-based categories, sufficient cognitive resources are needed to test hypotheses, maintain the currently active rule in working memory, update rules after feedback, and to select a new rule if necessary. Prior research has demonstrated that conjunctive rules are more complex than unidimensional rules and place greater demands on executive functions like working memory. In our study, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded while participants performed a conjunctive rule-based category learning task with trial-by-trial feedback. In line with prior research, correct categorization responses resulted in a larger stimulus-locked late positive complex compared to incorrect responses, possibly indexing the updating of rule information in memory. Incorrect trials elicited a pronounced feedback-locked P300 elicited which suggested a disconnect between perception, and the rule-based strategy. We also examined the differential processing of stimuli that were able to be correctly classified by the suboptimal single-dimensional rule (“easy” stimuli) versus those that could only be correctly classified by the optimal, conjunctive rule (“difficult” stimuli). Among strong learners, a larger, late positive slow wave emerged for difficult compared with easy stimuli, suggesting differential processing of category items even though strong learners performed well on the conjunctive category set. Overall, the findings suggest that ERP combined with computational modelling can be used to better understand the cognitive processes involved in rule-based category learning.

[1]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  Multidimensional Models of Perception and Cognition , 2014 .

[2]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Category learning and the memory systems debate , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[3]  M. Riesenhuber,et al.  Categorization Training Results in Shape- and Category-Selective Human Neural Plasticity , 2007, Neuron.

[4]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[5]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory , 1998, Nature.

[6]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Cortical and subcortical brain regions involved in rule-based category learning , 2005, Neuroreport.

[7]  Julia Herschensohn,et al.  Age-related effects , 2013 .

[8]  F. Ashby,et al.  The effects of concurrent task interference on category learning: Evidence for multiple category learning systems , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  Christopher Canty Task switching in people with Parkinson's disease: the role of the basal ganglia , 2012 .

[10]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  After the P3: late executive processes in stimulus categorization. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[11]  W Todd Maddox,et al.  Development of implicit and explicit category learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Heterarchical reinforcement-learning model for integration of multiple cortico-striatal loops: fMRI examination in stimulus-action-reward association learning , 2006, Neural Networks.

[13]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[14]  M. Petrides,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  J. P. Minda,et al.  Familiarization May Minimize Age-Related Declines in Rule-Based Category Learning , 2017, Psychology and aging.

[16]  W. Todd Maddox,et al.  Rule-based and information-integration category learning in normal aging , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  R. Cameron Craddock,et al.  Predicting intrinsic brain activity , 2013, NeuroImage.

[18]  W Todd Maddox,et al.  Removing the Frontal Lobes , 2010, Psychological science.

[19]  J. P. Minda,et al.  Category learning in older adulthood: A study of the Shepard, Hovland, and Jenkins (1961) tasks. , 2016, Psychology and aging.

[20]  P. J. Reber,et al.  A review of medial temporal lobe and caudate contributions to visual category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  R. Romo,et al.  Functional properties of primate putamen neurons during the categorization of tactile stimuli. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Category learning in the brain. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  R. Morrison,et al.  Dissociation of category-learning systems via brain potentials , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[24]  Rahel Rabi,et al.  Rule-Based Category Learning in Children: The Role of Age and Executive Functioning , 2014, PloS one.

[25]  Daniel S. Ruchkin,et al.  11 Positive Slow Wave and P300: Association and Disassociation , 1983 .

[26]  A. Kok Effects of degradation of visual stimuli on components of the event-related potential (ERP) in go/nogo reaction tasks , 1986, Biological Psychology.

[27]  J. P. Minda,et al.  Ego depletion interferes with rule-defined category learning but not non-rule-defined category learning , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[28]  F. Ashby,et al.  Category learning and multiple memory systems , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  John Paul Minda,et al.  Learning rule-described and non-rule-described categories: a comparison of children and adults. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  Haline E. Schendan,et al.  Object-sensitive activity reflects earlier perceptual and later cognitive processing of visual objects between 95 and 500ms , 2010, Brain Research.

[31]  W T Maddox,et al.  Comparing decision bound and exemplar models of categorization , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Alternative strategies of categorization , 1998, Cognition.

[33]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Jacqueline A. Rushby,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of phasic and tonic measures of the orienting reflex , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[35]  Sarah J. Miles,et al.  The effects of concurrent verbal and visual tasks on category learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  M. L. Kietzman,et al.  Slow wave and P300 in signal detection. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Multiple systems of category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[38]  A Kok,et al.  Effects of visual field, stimulus degradation, and level of practice on event-related potentials of the brain. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[39]  Rahel Rabi,et al.  Learning categories via rules and similarity: comparing adults and children. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[40]  A. Kok,et al.  Components of the event-related potential following degraded and undegraded visual stimuli , 1980, Biological Psychology.

[41]  Murray Grossman,et al.  The Neural Basis for Category-Specific Knowledge: An fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[42]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Toward a functional categorization of slow waves. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[44]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Dissociation between Striatal Regions while Learning to Categorize via Feedback and via Observation , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Ray Johnson P300: A Model of the Variables Controlling Its Amplitude a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[46]  E. Donchin,et al.  Parsing the late positive complex: mental chronometry and the ERP components that inhabit the neighborhood of the P300. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[47]  Stephen D. Mayhew,et al.  Article Learning Shapes the Representation of Behavioral Choice in the Human Brain , 2022 .

[48]  Shawn W. Ell,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Rule-based and Information-integration Category Learning , 2022 .

[49]  Age-related effects of different types of noise and stimulus quality: an event-related potential (ERP) study , 1996 .

[50]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Dual-task interference in perceptual category learning , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[51]  Ziv M. Williams,et al.  Selective enhancement of associative learning by microstimulation of the anterior caudate , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  How do the basal ganglia contribute to categorization? Their roles in generalization, response selection, and learning via feedback , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[53]  J. P. Minda,et al.  Better Mood and Better Performance , 2010, Psychological science.

[54]  G. Mulder,et al.  Language, memory, and aging: an electrophysiological exploration of the N400 during reading of memory-demanding sentences. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[55]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[56]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[57]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  THE ROLE OF THE BASAL GANGLIA IN CATEGORY LEARNING , 2006 .

[58]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. , 2005, Psychophysiology.