Selecting a response in task switching: testing a model of compound cue retrieval.

How can a task-appropriate response be selected for an ambiguous target stimulus in task-switching situations? One answer is to use compound cue retrieval, whereby stimuli serve as joint retrieval cues to select a response from long-term memory. In the present study, the authors tested how well a model of compound cue retrieval could account for a complex pattern of congruency effects arising from a procedure in which a cue, prime, and target were presented on each trial. A comparison of alternative models of prime-based effects revealed that the best model was one in which all stimuli participated directly in the process of retrieving a response, validating previous modeling efforts. Relations to current theorizing about response congruency effects and models of response selection in task switching are discussed.

[1]  Helen Arnold,et al.  Hitchhiker's guide to the galaxy , 2006, SIGGRAPH '06.

[2]  D. A. Taylor,et al.  The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Members of underrepresented groups: Reviewers for journal manuscripts wanted , 1989 .

[4]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  Adam N. Sanborn,et al.  Model evaluation using grouped or individual data , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  Thomas A. Schreiber,et al.  The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[7]  E. M. Altmann,et al.  Cue-independent task-specific representations in task switching: evidence from backward inhibition. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  S. Monsell Control of mental processes , 2021, Unsolved Mysteries of the Mind.

[9]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  U. Schimank,et al.  Diffusion and Random Walk Processes , 2022 .

[11]  E. M. Altmann,et al.  Task switching is not cue switching , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  R. West,et al.  Neural correlates of cue retrieval, task set reconfiguration, and rule mapping in the explicit cue task switching paradigm. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[13]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[14]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[16]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Task switching versus cue switching: using transition cuing to disentangle sequential effects in task-switching performance. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  N. Smelser,et al.  International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences , 2001 .

[18]  Stephan Lewandowsky,et al.  Using Parameter Sensitivity and Interdependence to Predict Model Scope and Falsifiability , 1996 .

[19]  I Koch,et al.  Automatic and intentional activation of task sets. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Mike Wendt,et al.  The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks , 2008, Psychological research.

[22]  N. Meiran Task Rule-Congruency and Simon-Like Effects in Switching between Spatial Tasks , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[23]  A. Kiesel,et al.  Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects , 2007, Psychological research.

[24]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[25]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[29]  U. Mayr Inhibition of action rules , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Priming cue encoding by manipulating transition frequency in explicitly cued task switching. , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  Claus Bundesen,et al.  Still clever after all these years: searching for the homunculus in explicitly cued task switching. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A retrieval theory of priming in memory. , 1988, Psychological review.

[33]  Thomas J. Palmeri,et al.  An Exemplar-Based Random Walk Model of Speeded Classification , 1997 .

[34]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Search of associative memory. , 1981 .

[35]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases “switch costs” in the explicit task-cuing procedure , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[36]  Frank Rösler,et al.  Is task switching nothing but cue priming? Evidence from ERPs , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[37]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[38]  J R Anderson,et al.  Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Interpreting instructional cues in task switching procedures: the role of mediator retrieval. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  An instance theory of attention and memory. , 2002, Psychological review.

[41]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[42]  M. Brass,et al.  Methodological and empirical issues when dissociating cue-related from task-related processes in the explicit task-cuing procedure , 2007, Psychological Research.

[43]  Catherine M. Arrington,et al.  Episodic and semantic components of the compound-stimulus strategy in the explicit task-cuing procedure , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[44]  B. Kantowitz,et al.  Partial advance information and stimulus dimensionality. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[45]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior , 1959 .

[46]  Claus Bundesen,et al.  Very clever homunculus: Compound stimulus strategies for the explicit task-cuing procedure , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[47]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. , 1997, Psychological review.

[48]  Harold Pashler,et al.  Procedural learning: II. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. , 1991 .

[49]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[50]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[51]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  "Schema Abstraction" in a Multiple-Trace Memory Model , 1986 .

[52]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  Separating cue encoding from target processing in the explicit task-cuing procedure: are there "true" task switch effects? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.