A robust asymmetry in loudness between rising- and falling-intensity tones

Tones rising in intensity over a few seconds are perceived as louder than symmetrical tones falling in intensity. However, the causes for such perceptual asymmetry, as well as its magnitude and dependency on contextual and methodological factors remain unclear. In this paper, two psychophysical experiments were conducted to measure the magnitude of this asymmetry for 2-s, 15-dB intensity-varying tones in different conditions. In the first experiment, participants assessed the global loudness of rising- and falling-intensity sounds with an absolute magnitude estimation procedure (AME); in the second experiment, they compared sounds relatively in an adaptive, two-interval, two-alternative forced-choice task (2I-2AFC). In both experiments, the region of intensity change, the design of experimental blocks, and the type of comparison stimulus were systematically manipulated to test for contextual and methodological factors. Remarkably, the asymmetry was virtually unaffected by the different contexts of presentation and similar results with 2I-2AFC and AME measurements were obtained. In addition, the size of the effect was comparable over all but the highest intensity regions (80–90 dB SPL), at which it was significantly smaller. All together, these results indicate that the loudness asymmetry is preserved under different measurement methods and contexts, and suggest that the underlying mechanism is strong and robust. In short, falling tones have to be about 4 dB higher in level than symmetrically rising tones in order to be perceived with the same global loudness, a finding that is still not predicted by current loudness models.

[1]  Michael Epstein,et al.  An introduction to induced loudness reduction. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  S Buus,et al.  Temporal integration of loudness as a function of level. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  R. Schlauch,et al.  The role of temporal-masking patterns in the determination of subjective duration and loudness for ramped and damped sounds. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Temporal and rate representations of time-varying signals in the auditory cortex of awake primates , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  L. Braida,et al.  Intensity perception. VII. Further data on roving-level discrimination and the resolution and bias edge effects. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  Birger Kollmeier,et al.  Loudness of complex time-varying sounds – A challenge for current Loudness models , 2013 .

[8]  L. Marks,et al.  Contextual processing of multidimensional and unidimensional auditory stimuli. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  M. Grassi,et al.  The subjective duration of audiovisual looming and receding stimuli , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[10]  N I Durlach,et al.  Intensity perception. XIII. Perceptual anchor model of context-coding. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  Anna Preis,et al.  Annoyance of Time-Varying Road-Traffic Noise , 2010 .

[12]  T. Irino,et al.  Temporal asymmetry in the auditory system. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  Daniel Oberfeld,et al.  Loudness changes induced by a proximal sound: loudness enhancement, loudness recalibration, or both? , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  W. M. Rabinowitz,et al.  Intensity perception. VI. Summary of recent data on deviations from Weber's law for 1000-Hz tone pulses. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  Ana Tajadura-Jiménez,et al.  Embodied auditory perception: the emotional impact of approaching and receding sound sources. , 2010, Emotion.

[16]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  Auditory looming perception in rhesus monkeys , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[18]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Looming Biases in Monkey Auditory Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  Neural Processing of Auditory Looming in the Human Brain , 2002, Current Biology.

[20]  H. Fastl,et al.  Dynamic Loudness Model (DLM) for Normal and Hearing-Impaired Listeners , 2002 .

[21]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  An Adaptive Bias in the Perception of Looming Auditory Motion , 2001 .

[22]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  Psychophysical scaling : Judgments of attributes or objects ? , 2015 .

[23]  W Jesteadt,et al.  An adaptive procedure for subjective judgments , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  Å. Hellström Time errors and differential sensation weighting. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  G. R. Patching,et al.  Time- and space-order effects in timed discrimination of brightness and size of paired visual stimuli. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  L. Marks,et al.  Measurement of Loudness, Part II: Context Effects , 2011 .

[27]  Stephen McAdams,et al.  Loudness asymmetries for tones with increasing and decreasing levels using continuous and global ratings , 2005 .

[28]  Patrick Susini,et al.  Temporal loudness weights for sounds with increasing and decreasing intensity profiles. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  Interactions between test- and inducer-tone durations in induced loudness reduction. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[30]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  Influential companions: Effects of one strategy on another in the within-subjects designs of cognitive psychology. , 1982 .

[31]  Robert Teghtsoonian,et al.  Sweep-induced acceleration in loudness change and the “bias for rising intensities” , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  J. Wang,et al.  Response characteristics of primary auditory cortex neurons underlying perceptual asymmetry of ramped and damped sounds , 2014, Neuroscience.

[33]  M. Grassi,et al.  The subjective duration of ramped and damped sounds , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  R. Patterson,et al.  The responses of single units in the inferior colliculus of the guinea pig to damped and ramped sinusoids , 2001, Hearing Research.

[35]  Klaus Scheffler,et al.  Rising sound intensity: an intrinsic warning cue activating the amygdala. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Jan Rennies,et al.  Spectro-Temporal Weighting of Loudness , 2012, PloS one.

[37]  D. Shore,et al.  Exploring the Role of the Amplitude Envelope in Duration Estimation , 2014, Perception.

[38]  Julien Tardieu,et al.  Loudness change in response to dynamic acoustic intensity. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Michael Schutz,et al.  Surveying the Temporal Structure of Sounds Used in Music Perception , 2014 .

[40]  E C Poulton,et al.  Unwanted asymmetrical transfer effects with balanced experimental designs. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.

[41]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[42]  E R Hafter,et al.  An effect of temporal asymmetry on loudness. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[43]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  Perceptual bias for rising tones , 1998, Nature.

[44]  Å. Hellström Comparison is not just subtraction: Effects of time- and space-order on subjective stimulus difference , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  R. Hellman,et al.  Loudness, annoyance, and noisiness produced by single-tone-noise complexes. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[47]  Poulton Ec,et al.  Unwanted asymmetrical transfer effects with balanced experimental designs. , 1966 .

[48]  R. Bakeman Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[49]  D. Cross,et al.  Sequential dependencies and regression in psychophysical judgments , 1973 .

[50]  Daniel Pressnitzer,et al.  Hierarchical processing of temporal asymmetry in human auditory cortex , 2014, 2014 International Workshop on Pattern Recognition in Neuroimaging.

[51]  N I Durlach,et al.  Intensity perception. I. Preliminary theory of intensity resolution. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[52]  Powen Ru,et al.  Multiresolution spectrotemporal analysis of complex sounds. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[53]  R. Schlauch,et al.  Mechanisms Responsible for Differences in Perceived Duration for Rising-Intensity and Falling-Intensity Sounds , 2007 .

[54]  Roy D. Patterson,et al.  The sound of a sinusoid: Time‐interval models , 1994 .

[55]  B. Moore,et al.  A Model of Loudness Applicable to Time-Varying Sounds , 2002 .

[56]  S. Meunier,et al.  Asymmetry in perceived duration between up-ramp and down-ramp sounds as a function of duration. , 2014, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[57]  R. Schlauch,et al.  Duration discrimination and subjective duration for ramped and damped sounds. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[58]  Jan Rennies,et al.  Comparison of loudness models for time-varying sounds , 2010 .

[59]  Erich Seifritz,et al.  Looming sounds as warning signals: the function of motion cues. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[60]  Allan D. Pierce,et al.  Acoustical Society of America , 2005 .

[61]  Jan Rennies,et al.  Loudness of Speech and Speech-Like Signals , 2013 .

[62]  Kirk N. Olsen,et al.  Perceptual Overestimation of Rising Intensity: Is Stimulus Continuity Necessary? , 2010, Perception.