Exaggerated redundancy gain in the split brain: A hemispheric coactivation account

Recent studies of redundancy gain indicate that it is especially large when redundant stimuli are presented to different hemispheres of an individual without a functioning corpus callosum. This suggests the hypothesis that responses to redundant stimuli are speeded partly because both hemispheres are involved in the activation of the response. A simple formal model incorporating this idea is developed and then elaborated to account for additional related findings. Predictions of the latter model are in good qualitative agreement with data from a number of sources, and there is neuroanatomic and psychophysiological support for its underlying structure.

[1]  M Hallett,et al.  Dissociation of the pathways mediating ipsilateral and contralateral motor‐evoked potentials in human hand and arm muscles , 1999, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  K. Donner,et al.  Visual latency and brightness: An interpretation based on the responses of rods and ganglion cells in the frog retina , 1989, Visual Neuroscience.

[3]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Locus of the redundant-signals effect in bimodal divided attention: A neurophysiological analysis , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  S. Miller,et al.  Evidence of activity-dependent withdrawal of corticospinal projections during human development , 2001, Neurology.

[5]  I. Kanazawa,et al.  Interhemispheric facilitation of the hand motor area in humans , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  A. Milner,et al.  Reaction times to lateralized visual stimuli in callosal agenesis: Stimulus and response factors , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[8]  T Paus,et al.  Blindsight in hemispherectomized patients as revealed by spatial summation across the vertical meridian. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  M. Voss,et al.  Delay of the execution of rapid finger movement by magnetic stimulation of the ipsilateral hand-associated motor cortex , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  M Iacoboni,et al.  Parallel visuomotor processing in the split brain: cortico-subcortical interactions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  K Matsunami,et al.  Effects of stimulation of corpus callosum on precentral neuron activity in the awake monkey. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  S Yantis,et al.  Dividing attention between color and shape: Evidence of coactivation , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  J. Requin,et al.  Changes in neuronal activity of the monkey precentral cortex during preparation for movement. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  W S Maki,et al.  Information-Processing by Pigeons , 1972, Science.

[15]  Silvia Savazzi,et al.  Speeding Up Reaction Time with Invisible Stimuli , 2002, Current Biology.

[16]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Splitbrain Monkeys: Cerebral Control of Ipsilateral and Contralateral Arm, Hand, and Finger Movements , 1972, Science.

[17]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Timecourse of coactivation in bimodal divided attention , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Tests of Race Models for Reaction Time in Experiments with Asynchronous Redundant Signals , 1997, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[19]  M. Peters,et al.  The Parallel Brain: The Cognitive Neuroscience of the Corpus Callosum , 2004 .

[20]  M. A. Jeeves,et al.  A comparison of interhemispheric transmission times in acallosals and normals , 1969 .

[21]  Michael C Corballis,et al.  Hemispheric interactions in simple reaction time , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Paul M. Corballis,et al.  Redundancy gain in simple reaction time following partial and complete callosotomy , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  M. Wiesendanger,et al.  Different Ipsilateral Representations for Distal and Proximal Movements in the Sensorimotor Cortex: Activation and Deactivation Patterns , 2001, NeuroImage.

[24]  R Ulrich,et al.  Separate-activation models with variable base times: Testability and checking of cross-channel dependency , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  H Pashler,et al.  Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  A new look at reaction time estimates of interhemispheric transmission time , 1988 .

[27]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Neural activity in primary motor and dorsal premotor cortex in reaching tasks with the contralateral versus ipsilateral arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  J. Mordkoff 18. Selective attention and internal constraints: there is more to the flanker effect than biased contingencies , 1996 .

[29]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  A. Leventhal,et al.  Signal timing across the macaque visual system. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  W. Schwarz,et al.  Diffusion, superposition, and the redundant-targets effect , 1994 .

[32]  P. Praamstra,et al.  Redundant-signals effects on reaction time, response force, and movement-related potentials in Parkinson's disease , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  M. Hallett,et al.  Role of the Ipsilateral Motor Cortex in Voluntary Movement , 1997, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[34]  M. Corballis Interhemispheric neural summation in the absence of the corpus callosum. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.

[36]  A. Urbano,et al.  Dynamic functional coupling of high resolution EEG potentials related to unilateral internally triggered one-digit movements. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Primary motor cortex is involved in bimanual coordination , 1998, Nature.

[38]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Cerebral control of contralateral and ipsilateral arm, hand and finger movements in the split-brain rhesus monkey. , 1973, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Ann Reynolds,et al.  The locus of redundant-targets and nontargets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  R. Ratcheson,et al.  Human Motor Cortex: Sensory Input Data from Single Neuron Recordings , 1972, Science.

[41]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Locating the Motor Cortex on the MRI with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation and PET , 1996, NeuroImage.

[42]  R. Cowie,et al.  The perception of structure , 1997 .

[43]  James T. Townsend,et al.  Serial exhaustive models can violate the race model inequality: Implications for architecture and capacity. , 1997 .

[44]  A M Wing,et al.  Brief bimanual force pulses: correlations between the hands in force and time. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  G. Logan,et al.  Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention , 1996 .

[46]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  An fMRI study of the human cortical motor system response to increasing functional demands. , 1997, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[47]  E. Zaidel,et al.  Simple reaction times to lateralized light flashes. Varieties of interhemispheric communication routes. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  Eugene Galanter,et al.  Handbook of mathematical psychology: I. , 1963 .

[49]  H Shibasaki,et al.  Movement-related potentials associated with bilateral simultaneous and unilateral movements recorded from human supplementary motor area. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[50]  H Shibasaki,et al.  Enhanced negative slope of cortical potentials before sequential as compared with simultaneous extensions of two fingers. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[51]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale , 1965, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[52]  M. Moscovitch The Development of Lateralization of Language Functions and Its Relation to Cognitive and Linguistic Development: A Review and Some Theoretical Speculations , 1977 .

[53]  G R Grice,et al.  Combination rule for redundant information in reaction time tasks with divided attention , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[54]  J. Townsend,et al.  Spatio-temporal properties of elementary perception: an investigation of parallel, serial, and coactive theories , 1995 .

[55]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  The asymmetrical brain , 2003 .

[56]  E. Franz,et al.  The Effect of Callosotomy on Novel Versus Familiar Bimanual Actions: A Neural Dissociation Between Controlled and Automatic Processes? , 2000, Psychological science.

[57]  M. Honda,et al.  Both primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area play an important role in complex finger movement. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  V. Swayze,et al.  Two Hemispheres—One Brain: Functions of the Corpus Callosum , 1987 .

[59]  Michael C Corballis,et al.  Interhemispheric neural summation in the split brain with symmetrical and asymmetrical displays , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  Claudio Babiloni,et al.  Responses of human primary sensorimotor and supplementary motor areas to internally triggered unilateral and simultaneous bilateral one‐digit movements. A high‐resolution EEG study , 1998 .

[61]  M Iacoboni,et al.  Channels of the corpus callosum. Evidence from simple reaction times to lateralized flashes in the normal and the split brain. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  E. J. Potchen,et al.  Cortical activation during rhythmic hand movements performed under three types of control: An fMRI study , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[63]  T. Bashore,et al.  Vocal and manual reaction time estimates of interhemispheric transmission time. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.

[64]  Paul M. Corballis,et al.  Paradoxical Interhemispheric Summation in the Split Brain , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  J Miller,et al.  Absence of coactivation in the motor component: evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[66]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Precentral projections to different parts of the spinal intermediate zone in therhesus monkey. , 1970, Brain research.

[67]  Howard E. Egeth,et al.  Redundancy gain revisited: Evidence for parallel processing of separable dimensions. , 1991 .

[68]  W Schwarz,et al.  Relationship between flanker identifiability and compatibility effect , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[69]  Steven Yantis,et al.  An interactive race model of divided attention. , 1991 .

[70]  J. Tanji,et al.  Neuronal activity in cortical motor areas related to ipsilateral, contralateral, and bilateral digit movements of the monkey. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[71]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Fate of neglected targets: a chronometric analysis of redundant target effects in the bisected brain. , 1995 .

[72]  M Giray,et al.  Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[73]  H. Hoff,et al.  SPINAL TERMINATIONS OF THE PROJECTION FIBRES FROM THE MOTOR CORTEX OF PRIMATES , 1934 .

[74]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[75]  H. Mushiake,et al.  An output zone of the monkey primary motor cortex specialized for bilateral hand movement , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  H. Asanuma,et al.  Effects of transcallosal volleys on pyramidal tract cell activity of cat. , 1962, Journal of neurophysiology.

[77]  R. Passingham,et al.  Temporary interference in human lateral premotor cortex suggests dominance for the selection of movements. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[78]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Neural interactions between motor cortical hemispheres during bimanual and unimanual arm movements , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[79]  D. Raab DIVISION OF PSYCHOLOGY: STATISTICAL FACILITATION OF SIMPLE REACTION TIMES* , 1962 .

[80]  D. Perani,et al.  Interhemispheric transmission of visuomotor information in humans: fMRI evidence. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[81]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Crossed–uncrossed difference in simple reaction times to lateralized flashes: between- and within-subjects variability , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[82]  W Schwarz,et al.  A new model to explain the redundant-signals effect , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[83]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology , 1995 .

[84]  Redundancy gains for visual search after complete commissurotomy. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[85]  J. Guillemot,et al.  Visual receptive field properties of cells innervated through the corpus callosum in the cat , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[86]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Intersensory facilitation in the motor component? , 1987 .

[87]  M Hallett,et al.  Source analysis of scalp-recorded movement-related electrical potentials. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[88]  Maureen Dennis,et al.  Impaired sensory and motor differentiation with corpus callosum agenesis: A lack of callosal inhibition during ontogeny? , 1976, Neuropsychologia.

[89]  M. Corballis,et al.  Interhemispheric transmission times in the presence and absence of the forebrain commissures: effects of luminance and equiluminance , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[90]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Language development and neurological theory , 1977 .