Personalization and differentiation as moderators of triggered displaced aggression towards out-group targets

Two studies examined the reduction of triggered displaced aggression (TDA) via bottom-up processing modes of de-categorization. Participants were provoked by the experimenter and then interacted with an ostensible out-group member who either did or did not provide a second (triggering) provocation. Study 1 compared TDA toward a triggering out-group member who had previously been either differentiated from the out-group, made the focus of self-other comparison, or was in a no-information control condition. As predicted, both differentiation and self-other comparison reduced aggression relative to the control condition. Study 2 examined the effect of negative self-disclosure from the out-group target, and contrasted its effects with both self-other comparison with a negative other, and a no-information control condition. As predicted, triggered participants in the negative self-disclosure condition aggressed less than those triggered in the negative self-other comparison or no-information control conditions. The liking induced by self-disclosure mediated its aggression-reducing effect.

[1]  William C. Pedersen,et al.  Kicking the (Barking) Dog Effect: The Moderating Role of Target Attributes on Triggered Displaced Aggression , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[2]  N. Miller,et al.  Prejudice and Intergroup Attributions: The Role of Personalization and Performance Feedback , 2005 .

[3]  William C. Pedersen,et al.  Chewing on it can chew you up: effects of rumination on triggered displaced aggression. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  William C. Pedersen,et al.  A Theoretical Model of Triggered Displaced Aggression , 2003 .

[5]  Norman Miller,et al.  The out-group must not be so bad after all: the effects of disclosure, typicality, and salience on intergroup bias. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  N. Miller,et al.  Decategorization and the reduction of bias in the crossed categorization paradigm , 2001 .

[7]  M. Hewstone,et al.  Does Multiple Categorization Reduce Intergroup Bias? , 2001 .

[8]  William C. Pedersen,et al.  The moderating effect of trivial triggering provocation on displaced aggression. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  William C. Pedersen,et al.  Displaced aggression is alive and well: a meta-analytic review. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  N. Miller,et al.  A theoretical analysis of crossed categorization effects: A meta-analysis. , 1998 .

[11]  Thomas F. Pettigrew,et al.  Generalized Intergroup Contact Effects on Prejudice , 1997 .

[12]  C. Anderson,et al.  Hot Temperatures, Hostile Affect, Hostile Cognition, and Arousal: Tests of a General Model of Affective Aggression , 1995 .

[13]  L. Miller,et al.  Self-disclosure and liking: a meta-analytic review. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  J. Steel,et al.  Interpersonal Correlates of Trust and Self-Disclosure , 1991 .

[15]  N. Miller,et al.  Effects of situational aggression cues: a quantitative review. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  S. Schwartz,et al.  Intergroup aggression: its predictors and distinctness from in-group bias. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  J. Berg,et al.  Effects of Racial Similarity and Interviewer Intimacy in a Peer Counseling Analogue. , 1988 .

[18]  S. Worchel The influence of contextual variables on interpersonal spacing , 1986 .

[19]  O. John,et al.  Social categorization and behavioral episodes: A cognitive analysis of the effects of intergroup contact. , 1985 .

[20]  D. A. Wilder,et al.  Empirical contributions: Predictions of belief homogeneity and similarity following social categorization , 1984 .

[21]  D. O. Sears,et al.  The person-positivity bias. , 1983 .

[22]  Robert M. May,et al.  The evolution of cooperation , 1981, Science.

[23]  R. Baron Heightened sexual arousal and physical aggression: An extension to females , 1979 .

[24]  E. Donnerstein,et al.  Direct and vicarious censure in the control of interracial aggression. , 1978, Journal of personality.

[25]  P. Cozby Self-disclosure, reciprocity and liking. , 1972, Sociometry.

[26]  H. Tajfel,et al.  Social categorization and intergroup behaviour , 1971 .

[27]  M. Worthy,et al.  Self-disclosure as an exchange process. , 1969 .

[28]  C. Halverson,et al.  Self-disclosure and interpersonal functioning. , 1969, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[29]  N. Anderson Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  John Cohen Frustration and Aggression , 1944, Nature.

[31]  C. I. Hovland,et al.  Minor Studies of Aggression: VI. Correlation of Lynchings with Economic Indices , 1940 .

[32]  William C. Pedersen,et al.  The moderating effect of trigger intensity on triggered displaced aggression , 2005 .

[33]  Rupert Brown,et al.  An integrative theory of intergroup contact , 2005 .

[34]  Norman Miller,et al.  Personalization and the Promise of Contact Theory , 2002 .

[35]  J. Dovidio,et al.  Reducing Intergroup Bias: The Common Ingroup Identity Model , 2000 .

[36]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  A Continuum of Impression Formation, from Category-Based to Individuating Processes: Influences of Information and Motivation on Attention and Interpretation , 1990 .

[37]  Rupert Brown,et al.  Contact is not enough: An intergroup perspective on the 'contact hypothesis.' , 1986 .

[38]  M. Brewer,et al.  13 – Beyond the Contact Hypothesis: Theoretical Perspectives on Desegregation , 1984 .

[39]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Deindividuation and anger-mediated interracial aggression: Unmasking regressive racism. , 1981 .

[40]  Eugene Borgida,et al.  Sex stereotypes and social judgment , 1980 .

[41]  S. Cook,et al.  Interpersonal and Attitudinal Outcomes in Cooperating Interracial Groups. , 1978 .

[42]  H. Tajfel Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations. , 1978 .