Comparing Methods for Pairing Electrodes Across Ears With Cochlear Implants

Supplemental Digital Content is available in the text. Objectives: Currently, bilateral cochlear implants (CIs) are independently programmed in clinics using frequency allocations based on the relative location of a given electrode from the end of each electrode array. By pairing electrodes based on this method, bilateral CI recipients may have decreased sensitivity to interaural time differences (ITD) and/or interaural level differences (ILD), two cues critical for binaural tasks. There are multiple different binaural measures that can potentially be used to determine the optimal way to pair electrodes across the ears. Previous studies suggest that the optimal electrode pairing between the left and right ears may vary depending on the binaural task used. These studies, however, have only used one reference location or a single bilateral CI user. In both instances, it is difficult to determine if the results that were obtained reflect a measurement error or a systematic difference across binaural tasks. It is also difficult to determine from these studies if the differences between the three cues vary across electrode regions, which could result from differences in the availability of binaural cues across frequency regions. The purpose of this study was to determine if, after experience-dependent adaptation, there are systematic differences in the optimal pairing of electrodes at different points along the array for the optimal perception of ITD, ILD, and pitch. Design: Data from seven bilateral Nucleus users was collected and analyzed. Participants were tested with ITD, ILD, and pitch-matching tasks using five different reference electrodes in one ear, spaced across the array. Comparisons were conducted to determine if the optimal bilateral electrode pairs systematically differed in different regions depending on whether they were measured based on ITD sensitivity, ILD sensitivity, or pitch matching, and how those pairs differed from the pairing in the participants’ clinical programs. Results: Results indicate that there was a significant difference in the optimal pairing depending on the cue measured, but only at the basal end of the array. Conclusion: The results suggest that optimal electrode pairings differ depending on the cue measured to determine optimal pairing, at least for the basal end of the array. This also suggests that the improvements seen when using optimally paired electrodes may be tied to the particular percept being measured both to determine electrode pairing and to assess performance, at least for the basal end of the array.

[1]  R. Litovsky,et al.  Interaural Time-Delay Sensitivity in Bilateral Cochlear Implant Users: Effects of Pulse Rate, Modulation Rate, and Place of Stimulation , 2009, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.

[2]  Justin M Aronoff,et al.  The use of interaural time and level difference cues by bilateral cochlear implant users. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  B. Laback,et al.  Perception and coding of interaural time differences with bilateral cochlear implants , 2015, Hearing Research.

[4]  Stefan Kerber,et al.  Sound Localization in Noise by Normal-Hearing Listeners and Cochlear Implant Users , 2012, Ear and hearing.

[5]  J. C. Middlebrooks Sound localization. , 2015, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[6]  Bruce J Gantz,et al.  Comparison of Speech Recognition and Localization Performance in Bilateral and Unilateral Cochlear Implant Users Matched on Duration of Deafness and Age at Implantation , 2008, Ear and hearing.

[7]  Richard Van Hoesel,et al.  Sound-Direction Identification, Interaural Time Delay Discrimination, and Speech Intelligibility Advantages in Noise for a Bilateral Cochlear Implant User , 2002, Ear and hearing.

[8]  Mathias Dietz,et al.  Comparison of Interaural Electrode Pairing Methods for Bilateral Cochlear Implants , 2015, Trends in hearing.

[9]  F. Wightman,et al.  The dominant role of low-frequency interaural time differences in sound localization. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  Justin M. Aronoff,et al.  Changing stimulation patterns can change the broadness of contralateral masking functions for bilateral cochlear implant users , 2018, Hearing Research.

[11]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  Justin M Aronoff,et al.  The Effect of Different Cochlear Implant Microphones on Acoustic Hearing Individuals' Binaural Benefits for Speech Perception in Noise , 2011, Ear and hearing.

[13]  B. Seeber,et al.  Localization cues with bilateral cochlear implants. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  Pavel Sovka,et al.  ACE Strategy with Virtual Channels , 2008 .

[15]  Becky B. Poon,et al.  Sensitivity to interaural time difference with bilateral cochlear implants: Development over time and effect of interaural electrode spacing. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  D. M. Green,et al.  Sound localization by human listeners. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  Alan Kan,et al.  Effect of mismatched place-of-stimulation on binaural fusion and lateralization in bilateral cochlear-implant users. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  R. Tyler,et al.  Speech perception, localization, and lateralization with bilateral cochlear implants. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  Mario A Svirsky,et al.  Bilateral cochlear implants with large asymmetries in electrode insertion depth: implications for the study of auditory plasticity , 2015, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[20]  Qian-Jie Fu,et al.  Speech perception with music maskers by cochlear implant users and normal-hearing listeners. , 2012, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[21]  R. Litovsky,et al.  Effect of channel separation and interaural mismatch on fusion and lateralization in normal-hearing and cochlear-implant listeners. , 2019, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  Bruce J Gantz,et al.  Bilateral and Unilateral Cochlear Implant Users Compared on Speech Perception in Noise , 2010, Ear and hearing.

[23]  Thomas Klenzner,et al.  Quality Control after Cochlear Implant Surgery by Means of Rotational Tomography , 2005, Otology & neurotology : official publication of the American Otological Society, American Neurotology Society [and] European Academy of Otology and Neurotology.

[24]  G. F. Kuhn Model for the interaural time differences in the azimuthal plane , 1977 .

[25]  Justin M. Aronoff,et al.  Perceptually aligning apical frequency regions leads to more binaural fusion of speech in a cochlear implant simulation , 2016, Hearing Research.

[26]  R. Plomp,et al.  Binaural speech intelligibility in noise for hearing-impaired listeners. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  Monica Padilla,et al.  Interleaved Processors Improve Cochlear Implant Patients’ Spectral Resolution , 2016, Ear and hearing.

[28]  Justin M Aronoff,et al.  Cochlear implant patients' localization using interaural level differences exceeds that of untrained normal hearing listeners. , 2012, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  Alan Kan,et al.  Effects of Interaural Pitch Matching and Auditory Image Centering on Binaural Sensitivity in Cochlear Implant Users , 2015, Ear and hearing.

[30]  Bruce J Gantz,et al.  Effects of Extreme Tonotopic Mismatches Between Bilateral Cochlear Implants on Electric Pitch Perception: A Case Study , 2011, Ear and hearing.

[31]  William F. House,et al.  Cochlear Implants: Histopathologic Findings Related to Performance in 16 Human Temporal Bones , 1991, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[32]  J. C. Steinberg,et al.  Factors Governing the Intelligibility of Speech Sounds , 1945 .

[33]  D. Rom A sequentially rejective test procedure based on a modified Bonferroni inequality , 1990 .

[34]  R. V. Hoesel Exploring the benefits of bilateral cochlear implants. , 2004 .

[35]  Bruce J Gantz,et al.  Cochlear Implant Speech Processor Frequency Allocations May Influence Pitch Perception , 2008, Otology & neurotology : official publication of the American Otological Society, American Neurotology Society [and] European Academy of Otology and Neurotology.

[36]  Frederick J. Gallun,et al.  The role of interaural differences on speech intelligibility in complex multi-talker environments. , 2017, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[37]  D. Grantham,et al.  Horizontal-Plane Localization of Noise and Speech Signals by Postlingually Deafened Adults Fitted With Bilateral Cochlear Implants* , 2007, Ear and hearing.

[38]  H Steven Colburn,et al.  Binaural sensitivity as a function of interaural electrode position with a bilateral cochlear implant user. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[39]  Justin M Aronoff,et al.  Pitch Matching Adapts Even for Bilateral Cochlear Implant Users with Relatively Small Initial Pitch Differences Across the Ears , 2019, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.