Orienteering experts report more proficient spatial processing and memory across adulthood

The closest surrogate to hunter-gather activity is the sport of orienteering, which naturally and simultaneously combines high-intensity interval exercise with navigation. Although human cognition can be improved across the lifespan through exercise and cognitive training, interventions like orienteering may be especially effective because they resemble activities engaged in by prehistoric humans during evolution. The present study tested whether orienteering experts have better hippocampal-dependent cognitive function than active, non-orienteering controls. One-hundred and fifty-eight healthy adults between the ages of 18 and 87 years old with varying experience in orienteering (none, intermediate, advanced, elite) reported on their spatial processing, spatial memory and episodic memory using the Navigational Strategy Questionnaire and the Survey of Autobiographical Memory. Orienteering experts reported greater use of allocentric and egocentric spatial processing and better spatial memory than controls. In contrast, episodic memory was not associated with orienteering expertise. Notably, the significant effects of orienteering on spatial cognition remained even after controlling for age, sex, and physical activity, suggesting that orienteering may be an effective intervention to prevent age-related cognitive decline in spatial navigation and memory.

[1]  Yang Liu,et al.  Shedding Light on the Effects of Orienteering Exercise on Spatial Memory Performance in College Students of Different Genders: An fNIRS Study , 2022, Brain sciences.

[2]  P. Sarasso,et al.  A Virtual Navigation Training Promotes the Remapping of Space in Allocentric Coordinates: Evidence From Behavioral and Neuroimaging Data , 2022, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.

[3]  C. Sanchez,et al.  Metacognitive accuracy across cognitive and physical task domains , 2022, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[4]  M. Avraamides,et al.  Comparable performance on a spatial memory task in data collected in the lab and online , 2021, PloS one.

[5]  C. Meneghetti,et al.  Orienteering: What relation with visuospatial abilities, wayfinding attitudes, and environment learning? , 2021, Applied Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  A. Lampit,et al.  Combined physical and cognitive training for older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials , 2020, Ageing Research Reviews.

[7]  A. Arleo,et al.  Differential Brain Activity in Regions Linked to Visuospatial Processing During Landmark-Based Navigation in Young and Healthy Older Adults , 2020, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.

[8]  Véronique D. Bohbot,et al.  Habitual use of GPS negatively impacts spatial memory during self-guided navigation , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[9]  Barbara Fenesi,et al.  The effects of aerobic exercise intensity on memory in older adults. , 2020, Applied physiology, nutrition, and metabolism = Physiologie appliquee, nutrition et metabolisme.

[10]  Morgan D Barense,et al.  Cognitive mapping style relates to posterior–anterior hippocampal volume ratio , 2019, Hippocampus.

[11]  Wang Bo,et al.  Effects of combined intervention of physical exercise and cognitive training on cognitive function in stroke survivors with vascular cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial , 2018, Clinical rehabilitation.

[12]  Maddalena Boccia,et al.  Is Losing One's Way a Sign of Cognitive Decay? Topographical Memory Deficit as an Early Marker of Pathological Aging. , 2019, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[13]  Toru Ishikawa,et al.  Satellite Navigation and Geospatial Awareness: Long-Term Effects of Using Navigation Tools on Wayfinding and Spatial Orientation , 2018, The Professional Geographer.

[14]  Michael Hornberger,et al.  Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Processing in Middle-Aged Adults at High Risk of Late-Onset Alzheimer's Disease: The PREVENT Dementia Study. , 2018, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[15]  Alessandro O. Caffò,et al.  Topographical disorientation in aging. Familiarity with the environment does matter , 2018, Neurological Sciences.

[16]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  The Primacy of Spatial Context in the Neural Representation of Events , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  J. Laczó,et al.  Subjective Spatial Navigation Complaints - A Frequent Symptom Reported by Patients with Subjective Cognitive Decline, Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer's Disease. , 2017, Current Alzheimer research.

[18]  H. Spiers,et al.  Global Determinants of Navigation Ability , 2017, Current Biology.

[19]  Suzanna Becker,et al.  The Effects of Physical Exercise and Cognitive Training on Memory and Neurotrophic Factors , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: A systematic review , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  W. van den Brink,et al.  Lower-Risk Cannabis Use Guidelines: A Comprehensive Update of Evidence and Recommendations , 2017, American journal of public health.

[22]  G. Alexander,et al.  Adaptive Capacity: An Evolutionary Neuroscience Model Linking Exercise, Cognition, and Brain Health , 2017, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  Beatrix Emo,et al.  Hippocampal and prefrontal processing of network topology to simulate the future , 2017, Nature Communications.

[24]  Jorge Martín Gutiérrez,et al.  Virtual Technologies to Develop Visual-Spatial Ability in Engineering Students , 2017 .

[25]  Maria Kozhevnikov,et al.  Relating allocentric and egocentric survey-based representations to the self-reported use of a navigation strategy of egocentric spatial updating , 2016 .

[26]  Roger McKinlay,et al.  Technology: Use or lose our navigation skills , 2016, Nature.

[27]  D. Lieberman Is Exercise Really Medicine? An Evolutionary Perspective , 2015, Current sports medicine reports.

[28]  Lars Bäckman,et al.  A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial , 2015, The Lancet.

[29]  Nicolas Cherbuin,et al.  A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal hippocampal atrophy in healthy human ageing , 2015, NeuroImage.

[30]  Panagiotis D Bamidis,et al.  Neuroplastic Effects of Combined Computerized Physical and Cognitive Training in Elderly Individuals at Risk for Dementia: An eLORETA Controlled Study on Resting States , 2015, Neural plasticity.

[31]  G. Riva,et al.  The role of egocentric and allocentric abilities in Alzheimer's disease: A systematic review , 2014, Ageing Research Reviews.

[32]  Fiona Barnett,et al.  Effects of combined cognitive and exercise interventions on cognition in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review , 2014, Ageing Research Reviews.

[33]  H. Abdi,et al.  The survey of autobiographical memory (SAM): A novel measure of trait mnemonics in everyday life , 2013, Cortex.

[34]  J. Saury,et al.  Accurately locating one's spatial position in one's environment during a navigation task: Adaptive activity for finding or setting control flags in orienteering , 2013 .

[35]  Véronique D. Bohbot,et al.  Spatial navigational strategies correlate with gray matter in the hippocampus of healthy older adults tested in a virtual maze , 2013, Front. Ag. Neurosci..

[36]  S. Gauggel,et al.  Do subjective measures of attention and memory predict actual performance? Metacognition in older couples. , 2012, Psychology and aging.

[37]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Spatial navigation training protects the hippocampus against age-related changes during early and late adulthood , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[38]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Cortical thickness changes following spatial navigation training in adulthood and aging , 2012, NeuroImage.

[39]  S. Moffat,et al.  Effects of age on navigation strategy , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[40]  Jakub Hort,et al.  Spatial navigation impairment is proportional to right hippocampal volume , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  Shane M. O'Mara,et al.  Aerobic exercise improves hippocampal function and increases BDNF in the serum of young adult males , 2011, Physiology & Behavior.

[42]  J. Barton,et al.  Age and gender differences in various topographical orientation strategies , 2011, Brain Research.

[43]  E. McAuley,et al.  Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Gerd Kempermann,et al.  Why and How Physical Activity Promotes Experience-Induced Brain Plasticity , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[45]  Jeffrey N. Browndyke,et al.  Aerobic Exercise and Neurocognitive Performance: A Meta-Analytic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials , 2010, Psychosomatic medicine.

[46]  Ricardo Nitrini,et al.  Topographical disorientation in Alzheimer's disease. , 2009, Arquivos de neuro-psiquiatria.

[47]  Nick C. Fox,et al.  A meta-analysis of hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer's disease , 2009, Neurobiology of Aging.

[48]  A. Kramer,et al.  Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  Carl W. Cotman,et al.  Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[50]  S. Becker,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. , 2007, Psychological review.

[51]  S. Resnick,et al.  Age differences in the neural systems supporting human allocentric spatial navigation , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[52]  David W. Eccles,et al.  Visual attention in orienteers at different levels of experience , 2006, Journal of sports sciences.

[53]  H. Braak,et al.  Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes , 2004, Acta Neuropathologica.

[54]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[55]  A. Kramer,et al.  Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults , 2003, Psychological science.

[56]  S. Resnick,et al.  Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[57]  C. Cotman,et al.  Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity , 2002, Trends in Neurosciences.

[58]  David W. Eccles,et al.  A grounded theory of expert cognition in orienteering. , 2002 .

[59]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[60]  P. Burns,et al.  Navigation and the mobility of older drivers. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[61]  R. Seiler Cognitive Processes in Orienteering: A Review. , 1996 .

[62]  R. Kesner,et al.  Caudate nucleus and memory for egocentric localization. , 1988, Behavioral and neural biology.