2 Car talk , car listen

Among age-old speculations about the origins of human language is a suggestion that use of the oral channel evolved to leave the hands and the rest of the body free for other activities. Whether this is true or not, there are few human activities that proceed unaccompanied by talking or listening. With the scope of everyday activities expanding to include some that are life-threatening unless performed with sufficient care, questions about the demands of language use on attention to other things have assumed new priority. This new priority runs up against an old, unresolved, but theoretically central psycholinguistic debate over how language production and language comprehension are related to each other. The terms of this relationship involve shared or divided components of linguistic knowledge and shared or divided resources of perceptual, motor, and cognitive skill. Our question in this chapter is how production and comprehension differ in their demands on attention or working memory (Baddeley, 2003), as reflected in how much production and comprehension interfere with other tasks. At bottom, we are interested in whether talking is harder than listening. The automatic answer to this question seems to be " of course ". Talking involves retrieving linguistic information from memory and assembling utterances in working memory, using only information from the intended meaning; listening benefits from specific form cues and accompanying recognition processes. The retrieval challenges for speakers are imposing: The estimated vocabulary of an educated English speaker is over 45,000 words. Failures of retrieval are common enough to have inspired a famous observation of William James (1890) about tip-of-the-tongue states and a large research literature on the phenomenon, beginning with Brown and McNeill (1966). Assembling utterances entails structuring and ordering the words into one of the infinite number of possible phrases or sentences of English, drawing on the 12,000-or-more syllables of which English words are composed , and articulating the syllables at a rate of three or four per second, using more muscle fibres than any other mechanical performance of the human body (Fink, 1986). It is ability to speak a foreign language, not ability to understand one, that constitutes the commonplace standard of bilingual competence: Every word uttered, indeed every sound, is on display for

[1]  J. Urry Complexity , 2006, Interpreting Art.

[2]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  J. Fodor,et al.  The active use of grammar in speech perception , 1966 .

[4]  A J McKnight,et al.  The effect of cellular phone use upon driver attention. , 1993, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[5]  J. Huttenlocher The origins of language comprehension. , 1974 .

[6]  Thomas G. Bever,et al.  Sentence Comprehension: The Integration of Habits and Rules , 2001 .

[7]  K. Bock Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production , 1992 .

[8]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[9]  E. Zurif,et al.  Sentence processing and the mental representation of verbs , 1987, Cognition.

[10]  V. M. Holmes,et al.  Planning units and syntax in sentence production , 1978, Cognition.

[11]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Suppressing Where but Not What: , 2004, Psychological science.

[12]  T. Shallice,et al.  Isolating Cognitive Modules with the Dual-Task Paradigm: Are Speech Perception and Production Separate Processes? , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Putting first things first , 2004 .

[14]  H Alm,et al.  Changes in driver behaviour as a function of handsfree mobile phones--a simulator study. , 1994, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[15]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Sentence production: from mind to mouth , 1995 .

[16]  M. A. Recarte,et al.  Mental workload while driving: effects on visual search, discrimination, and decision making. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[17]  Steven C Cramer,et al.  Age and Features of Movement Influence Motor Overflow , 2003, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[18]  H. Benedict,et al.  Early lexical development: comprehension and production , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[19]  Donald G. MacKay,et al.  The organization of perception and action. A theory for language and other cognitive skills , 1987, The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences.

[20]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  The Effects of Speech Production and Speech Comprehension on Simulated Driving Performance , 2006 .

[21]  Robert Graham,et al.  Voice dialling can reduce the interference between concurrent tasks of driving and phoning , 2001 .

[22]  R. L. Solso Theories in cognitive psychology : the Loyola symposium , 1975 .

[23]  H Summala,et al.  Cognitive load and detection thresholds in car following situations: safety implications for using mobile (cellular) telephones while driving. , 1999, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[24]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Minding the clock , 2003 .

[25]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Independence of Input and Output Phonology in Word Processing and Short-Term Memory , 1999 .

[26]  Michael E Rakauskas,et al.  Effects of naturalistic cell phone conversations on driving performance. , 2004, Journal of safety research.

[27]  Merrill F. Garrett,et al.  Remarks on the Architecture of Language Processing Systems , 2000 .

[28]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  J M Violanti,et al.  Cellular phones and fatal traffic collisions. , 1998, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[30]  D. Mitchell,et al.  The Effects of Context and Content on Immediate Processing in Reading , 1978 .

[31]  J. Jonides,et al.  On the Cost and Benefit of Cost and Benefit , 1984 .

[32]  C. Osgood,et al.  Hesitation Phenomena in Spontaneous English Speech , 1959 .

[33]  Randall Hendrick,et al.  Memory-Load Interference in Syntactic Processing , 2002, Psychological science.

[34]  Susan M. Garnsey,et al.  The Contributions of Verb Bias and Plausibility to the Comprehension of Temporarily Ambiguous Sentences , 1997 .

[35]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[36]  Ian Reid,et al.  Complexity science , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[37]  M. A. Recarte,et al.  COGNITIVE DEMANDS OF HANDS-FREE-PHONE CONVERSATION WHILE DRIVING , 2002 .

[38]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[39]  Joost Van de Weijer,et al.  Language input for word discovery , 1999 .

[40]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .

[41]  Håkan Alm,et al.  THE USE OF CAR PHONES AND CHANGES IN DRIVER BEHAVIOUR , 2001 .

[42]  A. Baddeley Working memory: looking back and looking forward , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  A. Caramazza How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .

[44]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone , 2001, Psychological science.

[45]  V. M. Holmes,et al.  Detection of extraneous signals during sentence recognition , 1970 .

[46]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Language processing: functional organization and neuroanatomical basis. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  R. Tibshirani,et al.  Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. , 1997, The New England journal of medicine.

[48]  Susan M. Garnsey,et al.  Agreement Processes in Sentence Comprehension , 1999 .

[49]  G. Dell,et al.  Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production , 2007, Cognition.

[50]  Marily Ford,et al.  A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences , 1983 .

[51]  D. Strayer,et al.  Cell phone-induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[52]  J M Violanti,et al.  Cellular phones and traffic accidents: an epidemiological approach. , 1996, Accident; analysis and prevention.