Detecting an orientation component in animal paths when the preferred direction is individual-dependent.

An orientation component leads to directionally biased paths, with major consequences in animal population redistribution. Classical orientation analyses, which focus on the overall direction of motion, are useless for detecting such a component when the preferred direction is not common to the whole population, but differs from one path to another. In-depth path analyses are required in this case. They consist of determining whether paths are more suitably represented as biased or unbiased random walks. The answer is not easy because most animals' paths show some forward persistence propensity that acts as a purely local directional bias and, hence, blurs the possible occurrence of an additional, consistent bias in a preferred direction. I highlight the key differences between biased and unbiased random walks and the different ways orientation mechanisms can generate a consistent directional bias. I then examine the strength and weakness of the available methods likely to detect it. Finally, I introduce a new procedure based on the backward evolution of the beeline distance, from the end of the path, which might correspond to a goal toward which the animal orients itself, to each of the animal's preceding locations. This new procedure proves to be very efficient, as it requires only a small sample of short paths for detecting a possible orientation component.

[1]  C. Patlak Random walk with persistence and external bias , 1953 .

[2]  J. Byers CORRELATED RANDOM WALK EQUATIONS OF ANIMAL DISPERSAL RESOLVED BY SIMULATION , 2001 .

[3]  James Smith,et al.  The Food Searching Behaviour of Two European Thrushes. Ii: the Adaptiveness of the Search Patterns , 1974 .

[4]  S. Benhamou How to reliably estimate the tortuosity of an animal's path: straightness, sinuosity, or fractal dimension? , 2004, Journal of theoretical biology.

[5]  C. Tchen Random Flight with Multiple Partial Correlations , 1952 .

[6]  G. Fraenkel,et al.  The Orientation of Animals, Kineses, Taxes and Compass Reactions, , 1941 .

[7]  R. Kitching,et al.  A simple simulation model of dispersal of animals among units of discrete habitats , 1971, Oecologia.

[8]  Daniel Fortin,et al.  Searching behavior and use of sampling information by free-ranging bison (Bos bison) , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[9]  Hans Scharstein Paths of Carabid Beetles Walking in the Absence of Orienting Stimuli and the Time Structure of their Motor Output , 1990 .

[10]  F. Bigler,et al.  An individual-based model of Trichogramma foraging behaviour : parameter estimation for single females , 1996 .

[11]  N. Hill,et al.  A biased random walk model for the trajectories of swimming micro-organisms. , 1997, Journal of theoretical biology.

[12]  C. Patrick Doncaster,et al.  Pelagic seabirds and the marine environment: foraging patterns of wandering albatrosses in relation to prey availability and distribution , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  C. McCulloch,et al.  Analyzing Discrete Movement Data as a Correlated Random Walk , 1989 .

[14]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  An olfactory orientation model for mammals' movements in their home ranges , 1989 .

[15]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Bicoordinate navigation based on non-orthogonal gradient fields. , 2003, Journal of theoretical biology.

[16]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Orientation and foraging movements in a patchy environment by the ant Serrastruma lujae (formicidae-myrmicinae) , 1993, Behavioural Processes.

[17]  H. Berg How Bacteria Swim , 1975 .

[18]  James Smith,et al.  The Food Searching Behaviour of Two European Thrushes , 1974 .

[19]  A. Hofgaard,et al.  Foraging and movement paths of female reindeer: insights from fractal analysis, correlated random walks, and Lévy flights , 2002 .

[20]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Successful homing of magnet-carrying white-chinned petrels released in the open sea , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  S. Benhamou,et al.  Spatial analysis of animals' movements using a correlated random walk model* , 1988 .

[22]  D. B. Siniff,et al.  A Simulation Model of Animal Movement Patterns , 1969 .

[23]  Balázs Kiss,et al.  Foraging in agricultural fields: local ‘sit-and-move’ strategy scales up to risk-averse habitat use in a wolf spider , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  Wolfgang Wiltschko,et al.  Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  S. Benhamou,et al.  How animals use their environment: a new look at kinesis , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  L. M. Marsh,et al.  The form and consequences of random walk movement models , 1988 .

[27]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  FAD: Fish Aggregating Device or Fish Attracting Device? A new analysis of yellowfin tuna movements around floating objects , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  R L Hall,et al.  Amoeboid movement as a correlated walk , 1977, Journal of mathematical biology.

[29]  E A Codling,et al.  Calculating spatial statistics for velocity jump processes with experimentally observed reorientation parameters , 2005, Journal of mathematical biology.

[30]  E A Codling,et al.  Sampling rate effects on measurements of correlated and biased random walks. , 2005, Journal of theoretical biology.

[31]  H. G. Wallraff,et al.  Avian olfactory navigation: its empirical foundation and conceptual state , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[32]  P. Kareiva,et al.  Analyzing insect movement as a correlated random walk , 1983, Oecologia.

[33]  Paul G. Blackwell,et al.  Random diffusion models for animal movement , 1997 .

[34]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[35]  Stephen P. Ellner,et al.  SCALING UP ANIMAL MOVEMENTS IN HETEROGENEOUS LANDSCAPES: THE IMPORTANCE OF BEHAVIOR , 2002 .

[36]  K. Mardia Statistics of Directional Data , 1972 .

[37]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  An analysis of movements of the wood mouse Apodemus sylvaticus in its home range , 1991, Behavioural Processes.

[38]  Stefano Focardi,et al.  Do ungulates exhibit a food density threshold? A field study of optimal foraging and movement patterns , 1996 .

[39]  D. Wilkinson,et al.  The Random Element in Bird ‘Navigation’ , 1952 .

[40]  Mario A. Natiello,et al.  THE INFLUENCE OF MOVEMENT AND RESTING BEHAVIOR ON THE RANGE OF THREE CARABID BEETLES , 1998 .

[41]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Spatial memory and searching efficiency , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  Michael L. Cain When Do Treatment Differences in Movement Behaviors Produce Observable Differences in Long-Term Displacements? , 1991 .

[43]  P Jouventin,et al.  Orientation in the wandering albatross: interfering with magnetic perception does not affect orientation performance , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[44]  G. Weiss,et al.  A descriptive theory of cell migration on surfaces. , 1974, Journal of theoretical biology.

[45]  J. A. Schaefer,et al.  Caribou movement as a correlated random walk , 2000, Oecologia.

[46]  Wolf M. Mooij,et al.  Search paths of swans foraging on spatially autocorrelated tubers , 2002 .

[47]  Patrick A. Zollner,et al.  Foray Search: An Effective Systematic Dispersal Strategy in Fragmented Landscapes , 2003, The American Naturalist.

[48]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Distinguishing between elementary orientation mechanisms by means of path analysis , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  W. H. Neill,et al.  Modelling animal movement as a persistent random walk in two dimensions: expected magnitude of net displacement , 2000 .