Internal models for bi-manual tasks.

Co-ordinated bi-manual actions form the basis for many everyday motor skills. In this review, the internal model approach to the problem of bi-manual co-ordination is presented. Bi-manual coordinative tasks are often regarded as a hallmark of complex action. They are often associated with object manipulation, whether the holding of a single object between the two hands or holding an object in each hand. However, the task of movement and control is deceptively difficult even when we execute an action with a single hand without holding an object. The simplest voluntary action requires the problems of co-ordination, timing and interaction between neural, muscular and skeletal structures to be overcome. When we are making a movement whilst holding an object, a further requirement is that an internal model is able to predict the dynamics of the object that is being held as well as the dynamics of the motor system. There has been extensive work examining the formation of internal models when acting in novel environments. The majority of studies examine uni-lateral learning of a task generally to the participant's dominant hand. However, many everyday motor tasks are bi-manual, and the existing findings regarding the learning of internal models in uni-manual tasks and their subsequent generalization highlights the complexities that must underlie the formation of bi-manual tasks. Our ability to perform bi-manual tasks raises interesting questions about how internal models are specified for co-ordinative actions, and also for how the motor system learns to represent the properties of objects.

[1]  R. Johansson,et al.  Control of grip force during restraint of an object held between finger and thumb: responses of muscle and joint afferents from the digits , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Postural adjustments associated with different unloadings of the forearm: effects of proprioceptive and cutaneous afferent deprivation. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[3]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—IV. Anticipatory smooth eye movements depend on prior target motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[4]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Two hands, one brain: cognitive neuroscience of bimanual skill , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  J. Massion,et al.  Acquisition of co-ordination between posture and movement in a bimanual task , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  D Goodman,et al.  On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  L. W. Stark,et al.  Feedforward stabilization in a bimanual unloading task , 1992, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  P. R. Davidson,et al.  Motor learning and prediction in a variable environment , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[9]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  David J. Ostry,et al.  A critical evaluation of the force control hypothesis in motor control , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  J. Massion,et al.  Postural forearm changes induced by predictable in time or voluntary triggered unloading in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[13]  Joachim Hermsdörfer,et al.  Sensorimotor memory and grip force control: does grip force anticipate a self‐produced weight change when drinking with a straw from a cup? , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size–weight illusion , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Some factors pertinent to the organization and control of arm movements , 1982, Brain Research.

[16]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Perspectives and problems in motor learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Roland S. Johansson,et al.  Sensorimotor interactions between pairs of fingers in bimanual and unimanual manipulative tasks , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  A. G. Witney,et al.  Learning and decay of prediction in object manipulation. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  J. Massion,et al.  Coordination between posture and movement in a bimanual load-lifting task: is there a transfer? , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[23]  R. Johansson,et al.  Somatosensory control of precision grip during unpredictable pulling loads , 1992, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  P Jenmalm,et al.  Visual and Somatosensory Information about Object Shape Control Manipulative Fingertip Forces , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Massion Movement, posture and equilibrium: Interaction and coordination , 1992, Progress in Neurobiology.

[27]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[29]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Randall Flanagan,et al.  Anticipatory postural adjustments in stance and grip , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  A. Wing,et al.  Predictive and reactive co‐ordination of grip and load forces in bimanual lifting in man , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[33]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Amy J. Bastian,et al.  Inter- and intra-limb generalization of adaptation during catching , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[36]  J. Neumann,et al.  Theory of games and economic behavior , 1945, 100 Years of Math Milestones.

[37]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Sensory-motor coordination during grasping and manipulative actions , 1992, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  R S Johansson,et al.  Grasp stability during manipulative actions. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[40]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Temporal and amplitude generalization in motor learning. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Motor prediction , 2001, Current Biology.

[42]  D. Wolpert Computational approaches to motor control , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  D M Wolpert,et al.  The influence of previous experience on predictive motor control , 2001, Neuroreport.

[44]  R. Johansson,et al.  Independent control of human finger‐tip forces at individual digits during precision lifting. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.

[45]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  MOSAIC Model for Sensorimotor Learning and Control , 2001, Neural Computation.

[46]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Modulation of grip force with load force during point-to-point arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip I: Basic coordination of force , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  H. Forssberg,et al.  TACTILE CONTROL OF ISOMETRIC FINGERTIP FORCES DURING GRASPING IN CHILDREN WITH CEREBRAL PALSY , 1995, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[50]  W. Hines,et al.  A multivariate solution for cyclic data, applied in modelling locomotor forces , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[51]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[52]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[53]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[54]  J. Massion,et al.  Anticipatory postural changes induced by active unloading and comparison with passive unloading in man , 1982, Pflügers Archiv.

[55]  J. Flanagan,et al.  The stability of precision grip forces during cyclic arm movements with a hand-held load , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  Konrad Paul Körding,et al.  The loss function of sensorimotor learning. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[57]  Yukari Ohki,et al.  Dependence of reactive responses in human bimanual finger movements on sensory feedback and auditory cues. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  A. G. Witney,et al.  Predictive motor learning of temporal delays. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  D. Harrington,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of movement , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[60]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Sensorimotor Memory For Fingertip Forces: Evidence For A Task-Independent Motor Memory , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  M. Kawato,et al.  A hierarchical neural-network model for control and learning of voluntary movement , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[63]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  D J Serrien,et al.  Bimanual organization of manipulative forces: evidence from erroneous feedforward programming of precision grip , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[65]  R. Johansson,et al.  Programmed and triggered actions to rapid load changes during precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  R. S. Johansson,et al.  Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[68]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Predicting the Consequences of Our Own Actions: The Role of Sensorimotor Context Estimation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  R. Johansson,et al.  Control of grip force during restraint of an object held between finger and thumb: responses of cutaneous afferents from the digits , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  R. Johansson,et al.  Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.

[71]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[72]  T D Lee,et al.  Acquiring bimanual skills: contrasting forms of information feedback for interlimb decoupling. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[73]  Michael I. Jordan Chapter 2 Computational aspects of motor control and motor learning , 1996 .

[74]  D. Elliott,et al.  The contribution of vision to asymmetries in manual aiming , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[75]  I Salimi,et al.  Specificity of internal representations underlying grasping. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[76]  Y. Ohki,et al.  Cutaneous inputs can activate the ipsilateral primary motor cortex during bimanual sensory-driven movements in humans. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[77]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Neural Control of Limb Mechanics for Visuomanual Coordination , 1996 .

[78]  Mario Wiesendanger,et al.  Dissociation of grip/load-force coupling during a bimanual manipulative assignment , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[79]  H. Freund,et al.  Invariant temporal characteristics of manipulative hand movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[80]  Roland S Johansson,et al.  Predictions Specify Reactive Control of Individual Digits in Manipulation , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[81]  R. Johansson,et al.  Mechanisms for force adjustments to unpredictable frictional changes at individual digits during two-fingered manipulation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[82]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[83]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.