Better Know When (Not) to Think Twice

Before approaching situations, individuals frequently imagine “what would happen, if . . . .” Such prefactual thought can promote confidence and facilitate behavior preparation when the upcoming situation can benefit from forethought, but it also delays action. The present research tested how social power predicts prefactual thought when its benefits are clear versus ambiguous. Power enhances flexible behavior adaptation and action tendencies—presumably without much forethought. We therefore proposed that power diminishes prefactual thought, unless the situation suggests that such thought is adaptive (i.e., could benefit performance). Power-holders indeed generated less prefactuals than the powerless (Experiments 1 and 2), but only if benefits for performance were ambiguous rather than clear (Experiment 3). These findings indicate that social context factors related to confidence affect prefactual thought, and that power-holders’ flexible adaptation to the situation sometimes elicits inaction (i.e., prefactual thought) rather than spontaneous action.

[1]  Klaus Jonas,et al.  Give a person power and he or she will show interpersonal sensitivity: the phenomenon and its why and when. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Nathanael J. Fast,et al.  Illusory Control: A Generative Force Behind Power's Far-Reaching Effects , 2008 .

[3]  Keith E. Niedermeier,et al.  What if I find it cheaper someplace else?: Role of prefactual thinking and anticipated regret in consumer behavior , 2000 .

[4]  Joe C Magee,et al.  Power and the objectification of social targets. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  S. Schulz-Hardt,et al.  "It's not my fault--but only I can change it": counterfactual and prefactual thoughts of managers. , 2004, The Journal of applied psychology.

[6]  Ezequiel Morsella,et al.  THE SPONTaNEOuS THOuGHTS Of THE NiGHT: HOW fuTurE TaSKS BrEEd iNTruSivE COGNiTiONS , 2010 .

[7]  Roderick M. Kramer,et al.  Leadership and the Psychology of Power , 2004 .

[8]  Christopher J. Holden,et al.  Self-Esteem Instability and Personality , 2015, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[9]  P. Niedenthal,et al.  Adult attachment and the perception of facial expression of emotion. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Ana Guinote,et al.  How long will it take? Power biases time predictions , 2010 .

[11]  Letitia Slabu,et al.  Power increases situated creativity , 2013 .

[12]  P. Tetlock,et al.  Accountability and Close-Call Counterfactuals: The Loser Who Nearly Won and the Winner Who Nearly Lost , 2000 .

[13]  Katie A. Liljenquist,et al.  The blind leading: Power reduces awareness of constraints , 2013 .

[14]  Marianne Schmid Mast,et al.  Dominance as expressed and inferred through speaking time: A meta-analysis , 2002 .

[15]  L. Sanna Defensive pessimism, optimism, and simulating alternatives: some ups and downs of prefactual and counterfactual thinking. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  P. Gollwitzer The Volitional Benefits of Planning , 1996 .

[17]  Jerome L. Myers,et al.  Research Design and Statistical Analysis , 1991 .

[18]  S. Fiske,et al.  Controlling other people. The impact of power on stereotyping. , 1993, The American psychologist.

[19]  Cameron Anderson,et al.  Power, Approach, and Inhibition , 2003 .

[20]  K. Sassenberg,et al.  Power in group contexts: the influence of group status on promotion and prevention decision making. , 2013, The British journal of social psychology.

[21]  L. Sanna Defensive Pessimism and Optimism: The Bitter-Sweet Influence of Mood on Performance and Prefactual and Counterfactual Thinking , 1998 .

[22]  M. Brauer,et al.  Social power , 2006 .

[23]  Judith A. Hall,et al.  Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: a meta-analysis. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  K. Sassenberg,et al.  Where could we stand if I had…? How social power impacts counterfactual thinking after failure , 2014 .

[25]  Naomi Ellemers,et al.  The attraction of social power: The influence of construing power as opportunity versus responsibility , 2012 .

[26]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  The Nonconscious Nature of Power: Cues and Consequences , 2010 .

[27]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles, 2nd ed. , 2007 .

[28]  K. Markman,et al.  A Reflection and Evaluation Model of Comparative Thinking , 2003, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[29]  John R. Anderson Retrieval of propositional information from long-term memory , 1974 .

[30]  John V. Petrocelli,et al.  Event detail and confidence in gambling: The role of counterfactual thought reactions , 2010 .

[31]  Ana Guinote,et al.  Power and affordances: when the situation has more power over powerful than powerless individuals. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  G. Oettingen,et al.  Turning Fantasies About Positive and Negative Futures into Self-Improvement Goals , 2005 .

[33]  Katie A. Liljenquist,et al.  Power reduces the press of the situation: implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  A. Guinote,et al.  Does Power Magnify the Expression of Dispositions? , 2012, Psychological science.

[35]  Ana Guinote,et al.  Behaviour variability and the Situated Focus Theory of Power , 2007 .

[36]  Sean M. McCrea,et al.  Dysfunctional anticipatory thoughts and the self‐handicapping strategy. , 2012 .

[37]  Ana Guinote,et al.  Power and Goal Pursuit , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[38]  Deborah A. Prentice,et al.  Contrast tests of interaction hypothesis. , 1997 .

[39]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  The Effects of Mindset on Behavior: Self-Regulation in Deliberative and Implemental Frames of Mind , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[40]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  You focus on the forest when you're in charge of the trees: power priming and abstract information processing. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  Richard P. Larrick,et al.  Power, competitiveness, and advice taking: Why the powerful don’t listen , 2012 .

[42]  N. Roese,et al.  The Functional Theory of Counterfactual Thinking , 2008, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[43]  Joe C. Magee,et al.  The Social Distance Theory of Power , 2013, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[44]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  Power, Propensity to Negotiate, and Moving First in Competitive Interactions , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[45]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  From Thought to Action: Effects of Process-Versus Outcome-Based Mental Simulations on Performance , 1999 .

[46]  Nathanael J. Fast,et al.  When the Boss Feels Inadequate , 2009, Psychological science.

[47]  J. Bargh,et al.  The psychology of action : linking cognition and motivation to behavior , 1999 .

[48]  Joe C Magee,et al.  From power to action. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  C. D. del Valle,et al.  Dispositional pessimism, defensive pessimism and optimism: The effect of induced mood on prefactual and counterfactual thinking and performance , 2008 .

[50]  James M. Olson,et al.  Outcome Controllability and Counterfactual Thinking , 1995 .

[51]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Short- and long-term memory retrieval: A comparison of the effects of information load and relatedness , 1987 .

[52]  Ana Guinote,et al.  Power affects basic cognition: Increased attentional inhibition and flexibility , 2007 .

[53]  Joseph B. Walther,et al.  Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication: Hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, language, and cognition , 2007, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[54]  Bernadette Park,et al.  Powerful perceivers, powerless objects: Flexibility of powerholders’ social attention☆ , 2006 .

[55]  N. Roese,et al.  Self-esteem and counterfactual thinking. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  Bernadette Park,et al.  When power does not corrupt: superior individuation processes among powerful perceivers. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[57]  G. Oettingen,et al.  Self-regulation of goal setting: turning free fantasies about the future into binding goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.