The body schema: neural simulation for covert and overt actions of embodied cognitive agents
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Shanahan. A cognitive architecture that combines internal simulation with a global workspace , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.
[2] Yoshiyuki Asai,et al. Correction: A Model of Postural Control in Quiet Standing: Robust Compensation of Delay-Induced Instability Using Intermittent Activation of Feedback Control , 2009, PLoS ONE.
[3] E. J. Capaldi,et al. A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[4] N. Bolognini,et al. Somatosensory cortical representation of the body size , 2019, Human brain mapping.
[5] A. G. Feldman. Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .
[6] Adrian J. T. Alsmith,et al. How do the body schema and the body image interact? , 2018, Consciousness and Cognition.
[7] Alice C. Roy,et al. Proprioception Is Necessary for Body Schema Plasticity: Evidence from a Deafferented Patient , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[8] H. Branch Coslett,et al. Evidence for Multiple, Distinct Representations of the Human Body , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] K. S. Lashley,et al. INTEGRATIVE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRAL CORTEX , 1933 .
[10] W. James,et al. The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .
[11] D. Vernon. Internal simulation in embodied cognitive systems: Comment on "Muscleless motor synergies and actions without movements: From motor neuroscience to cognitive robotics" by Vishwanathan Mohan et al. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.
[12] Kazuhiko Kawamura,et al. Implementation of Cognitive Control for a Humanoid Robot , 2008, Int. J. Humanoid Robotics.
[13] M. Jeannerod. Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.
[14] Maura Casadio,et al. Revisiting the Body-Schema Concept in the Context of Whole-Body Postural-Focal Dynamics , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[15] N. A. Bernshteĭn. The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .
[16] G. Hesslow. The current status of the simulation theory of cognition , 2012, Brain Research.
[17] P. Morasso,et al. Direct measurement of ankle stiffness during quiet standing: implications for control modelling and clinical application. , 2005, Gait & posture.
[18] Rick Grush,et al. The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[19] Philippe Gaussier,et al. Brain-Inspired Coding of Robot Body Schema Through Visuo-Motor Integration of Touched Events , 2019, Front. Neurorobot..
[20] R. A. Brooks,et al. Intelligence without Representation , 1991, Artif. Intell..
[21] Michael Beetz,et al. Envisioning the qualitative effects of robot manipulation actions using simulation-based projections , 2017, Artif. Intell..
[22] V. Braitenberg. Vehicles, Experiments in Synthetic Psychology , 1984 .
[23] Giulio Sandini,et al. Prospection in Cognition: The Case for Joint Episodic-Procedural Memory in Cognitive Robotics , 2015, Front. Robot. AI.
[24] G. Holmes,et al. Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions , 1911 .
[25] Giulio Sandini,et al. A Neural Framework for Organization and Flexible Utilization of Episodic Memory in Cumulatively Learning Baby Humanoids , 2014, Neural Computation.
[26] Maura Casadio,et al. A neural mechanism of synergy formation for whole body reaching , 2009, Biological Cybernetics.
[27] H. Maturana,et al. The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding , 2007 .
[28] Tomaso Poggio,et al. From Understanding Computation to Understanding Neural Circuitry , 1976 .
[29] Karl J. Friston,et al. Passive motion and active inference: Commentary on "Muscleless motor synergies and actions without movements: From motor neuroscience to cognitive robotics" by Vishwanathan Mohan, Ajaz Bhat and Pietro Morasso. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.
[30] John Schwoebel,et al. The man who executed “imagined” movements: Evidence for dissociable components of the body schema , 2002, Brain and Cognition.
[31] Sally A. Linkenauger,et al. Distortions of perceived volume and length of body parts , 2019, Cortex.
[32] F. Vignemont,et al. Beyond differences between the body schema and the body image: insights from body hallucinations , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[33] Karl J. Friston,et al. Action understanding and active inference , 2011, Biological Cybernetics.
[34] C. Hofsten. An action perspective on motor development , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[35] E. Naito,et al. Body schema as a link between motor control and cognitive function , 2012, 2012 ICME International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME).
[36] N. Hogan,et al. Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements? , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[37] M. Jeannerod. The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[38] Margaret Wilson,et al. Six views of embodied cognition , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[39] Taishin Nomura,et al. Bounded stability of the quiet standing posture: an intermittent control model. , 2008, Human movement science.
[40] A. Chemero. Radical Embodied Cognitive Science , 2009 .
[41] Alice C. Roy,et al. Tool use imagery triggers tool incorporation in the body schema , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[42] A. Maravita,et al. Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[43] A. Moran,et al. Does Motor Simulation Theory Explain the Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying Motor Imagery? A Critical Review , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[44] Norbert Wiener,et al. Cybernetics. , 1948, Scientific American.
[45] A. Farnè,et al. Rubber hand illusion modulates the influences of somatosensory and parietal inputs to the motor cortex. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.
[46] Pietro Morasso,et al. Muscleless motor synergies and actions without movements: From motor neuroscience to cognitive robotics. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.
[47] D. Wolpert,et al. Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[48] H. Branch Coslett,et al. Mental motor imagery and the body schema: evidence for proprioceptive dominance , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.
[49] Pietro Morasso,et al. Quiet standing: The Single Inverted Pendulum model is not so bad after all , 2019, PloS one.
[50] P. Morasso,et al. Kinematic networks , 1988, Biological Cybernetics.
[51] Alexandre Bernardino,et al. Online Body Schema Adaptation Based on Internal Mental Simulation and Multisensory Feedback , 2016, Front. Robot. AI.
[52] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[53] C. Spence,et al. Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach , 2003, Current Biology.
[54] R. Ptak,et al. The Dorsal Frontoparietal Network: A Core System for Emulated Action , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[55] Giorgio Metta,et al. Grounding vision through experimental manipulation , 2003, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.