The body schema: neural simulation for covert and overt actions of embodied cognitive agents

This brief commentary on the general topic of ‘body schema’ is focused on its computational role, as an internal model that integrates proprioceptive information, for allowing embodied cognitive agents to carry out the neural simulation of covert and overt actions in a unitary manner. The discussion takes inspiration from the vintage but still valid seminal observation by Marr and Poggio that, in order to understand cognitive agents, both human and artificial, we should consider them as Generalized Information Processing Systems, to be analyzed along three levels: computational, algorithmic, and implementation. Accordingly, the body schema concept is briefly analyzed along this line, with the purpose of outlining a cognitive architecture that links embodied cognition with motor control through the body schema.

[1]  M. Shanahan A cognitive architecture that combines internal simulation with a global workspace , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[2]  Yoshiyuki Asai,et al.  Correction: A Model of Postural Control in Quiet Standing: Robust Compensation of Delay-Induced Instability Using Intermittent Activation of Feedback Control , 2009, PLoS ONE.

[3]  E. J. Capaldi,et al.  A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  N. Bolognini,et al.  Somatosensory cortical representation of the body size , 2019, Human brain mapping.

[5]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[6]  Adrian J. T. Alsmith,et al.  How do the body schema and the body image interact? , 2018, Consciousness and Cognition.

[7]  Alice C. Roy,et al.  Proprioception Is Necessary for Body Schema Plasticity: Evidence from a Deafferented Patient , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  H. Branch Coslett,et al.  Evidence for Multiple, Distinct Representations of the Human Body , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  K. S. Lashley,et al.  INTEGRATIVE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRAL CORTEX , 1933 .

[10]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[11]  D. Vernon Internal simulation in embodied cognitive systems: Comment on "Muscleless motor synergies and actions without movements: From motor neuroscience to cognitive robotics" by Vishwanathan Mohan et al. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.

[12]  Kazuhiko Kawamura,et al.  Implementation of Cognitive Control for a Humanoid Robot , 2008, Int. J. Humanoid Robotics.

[13]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[14]  Maura Casadio,et al.  Revisiting the Body-Schema Concept in the Context of Whole-Body Postural-Focal Dynamics , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[15]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[16]  G. Hesslow The current status of the simulation theory of cognition , 2012, Brain Research.

[17]  P. Morasso,et al.  Direct measurement of ankle stiffness during quiet standing: implications for control modelling and clinical application. , 2005, Gait & posture.

[18]  Rick Grush,et al.  The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  Philippe Gaussier,et al.  Brain-Inspired Coding of Robot Body Schema Through Visuo-Motor Integration of Touched Events , 2019, Front. Neurorobot..

[20]  R. A. Brooks,et al.  Intelligence without Representation , 1991, Artif. Intell..

[21]  Michael Beetz,et al.  Envisioning the qualitative effects of robot manipulation actions using simulation-based projections , 2017, Artif. Intell..

[22]  V. Braitenberg Vehicles, Experiments in Synthetic Psychology , 1984 .

[23]  Giulio Sandini,et al.  Prospection in Cognition: The Case for Joint Episodic-Procedural Memory in Cognitive Robotics , 2015, Front. Robot. AI.

[24]  G. Holmes,et al.  Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions , 1911 .

[25]  Giulio Sandini,et al.  A Neural Framework for Organization and Flexible Utilization of Episodic Memory in Cumulatively Learning Baby Humanoids , 2014, Neural Computation.

[26]  Maura Casadio,et al.  A neural mechanism of synergy formation for whole body reaching , 2009, Biological Cybernetics.

[27]  H. Maturana,et al.  The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding , 2007 .

[28]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  From Understanding Computation to Understanding Neural Circuitry , 1976 .

[29]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Passive motion and active inference: Commentary on "Muscleless motor synergies and actions without movements: From motor neuroscience to cognitive robotics" by Vishwanathan Mohan, Ajaz Bhat and Pietro Morasso. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.

[30]  John Schwoebel,et al.  The man who executed “imagined” movements: Evidence for dissociable components of the body schema , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  Sally A. Linkenauger,et al.  Distortions of perceived volume and length of body parts , 2019, Cortex.

[32]  F. Vignemont,et al.  Beyond differences between the body schema and the body image: insights from body hallucinations , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[33]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Action understanding and active inference , 2011, Biological Cybernetics.

[34]  C. Hofsten An action perspective on motor development , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  E. Naito,et al.  Body schema as a link between motor control and cognitive function , 2012, 2012 ICME International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME).

[36]  N. Hogan,et al.  Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements? , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[37]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[38]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  Six views of embodied cognition , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  Taishin Nomura,et al.  Bounded stability of the quiet standing posture: an intermittent control model. , 2008, Human movement science.

[40]  A. Chemero Radical Embodied Cognitive Science , 2009 .

[41]  Alice C. Roy,et al.  Tool use imagery triggers tool incorporation in the body schema , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[42]  A. Maravita,et al.  Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  A. Moran,et al.  Does Motor Simulation Theory Explain the Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying Motor Imagery? A Critical Review , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[44]  Norbert Wiener,et al.  Cybernetics. , 1948, Scientific American.

[45]  A. Farnè,et al.  Rubber hand illusion modulates the influences of somatosensory and parietal inputs to the motor cortex. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Pietro Morasso,et al.  Muscleless motor synergies and actions without movements: From motor neuroscience to cognitive robotics. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.

[47]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  H. Branch Coslett,et al.  Mental motor imagery and the body schema: evidence for proprioceptive dominance , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[49]  Pietro Morasso,et al.  Quiet standing: The Single Inverted Pendulum model is not so bad after all , 2019, PloS one.

[50]  P. Morasso,et al.  Kinematic networks , 1988, Biological Cybernetics.

[51]  Alexandre Bernardino,et al.  Online Body Schema Adaptation Based on Internal Mental Simulation and Multisensory Feedback , 2016, Front. Robot. AI.

[52]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[53]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach , 2003, Current Biology.

[54]  R. Ptak,et al.  The Dorsal Frontoparietal Network: A Core System for Emulated Action , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Giorgio Metta,et al.  Grounding vision through experimental manipulation , 2003, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.