Newborn infants' auditory system is sensitive to Western music chord categories

Neural encoding of abstract rules in the audition of newborn infants has been recently demonstrated in several studies using event-related potentials (ERPs). In the present study the neural encoding of Western music chords was investigated in newborn infants. Using ERPs, we examined whether the categorizations of major vs. minor and consonance vs. dissonance are present at the level of the change-related mismatch response (MMR). Using an oddball paradigm, root minor, dissonant and inverted major chords were presented in a context of consonant root major chords. The chords were transposed to several different frequency levels, so that the deviant chords did not include a physically deviant frequency that could result in an MMR without categorization. The results show that the newborn infants were sensitive to both dissonant and minor chords but not to inverted major chords in the context of consonant root major chords. While the dissonant chords elicited a large positive MMR, the minor chords elicited a negative MMR. This indicates that the two categories were processed differently. The results suggest newborn infants are sensitive to Western music categorizations, which is consistent with the authors' previous studies in adults and school-aged children.

[1]  S. H. Hulse,et al.  Auditory discrimination of chord-based spectral structures by European starlings ( Sturnus vulgaris ) , 1995 .

[2]  R. Näätänen,et al.  A kind of auditory ‘primitive intelligence’ already present at birth , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  M Steinschneider,et al.  Consonance and dissonance of musical chords: neural correlates in auditory cortex of monkeys and humans. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  W. Fifer,et al.  Evidence of Transnatal Auditory Learning , 2000, Journal of Perinatology.

[5]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Maturation of the auditory event-related potentials during the first year of life , 2002, Neuroreport.

[6]  C. Drake,et al.  Tempo discrimination in infants , 1997 .

[7]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  Memory for melody: infants use a relative pitch code , 2005, Cognition.

[8]  Josh H. McDermott,et al.  Individual Differences Reveal the Basis of Consonance , 2010, Current Biology.

[9]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Neurobiological Foundations for the Theory of Harmony in Western Tonal Music , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[10]  S. Trehub,et al.  Tuning in to musical rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Feelings and Perceptions of Happiness and Sadness Induced by Music: Similarities, Differences, and Mixed Emotions , 2010 .

[12]  E. Hannon,et al.  Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: a cross-cultural comparison. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[13]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  Maturation of cortical mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  S. Trehub,et al.  Metrical Categories in Infancy and Adulthood , 2005, Psychological science.

[15]  C. Escera,et al.  The accuracy of sound duration representation in the human brain determines the accuracy of behavioural perception , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  Paavo H. T. Leppänen,et al.  The auditory sensory memory trace decays rapidly in newborns. , 2002, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[17]  P. Daston,et al.  Musical consonance as musical preference: a cross-cultural study. , 1968, The Journal of general psychology.

[18]  Hubert Preissl,et al.  Sound frequency change detection in fetuses and newborns, a magnetoencephalographic study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[19]  T. Rossing,et al.  The Science of Sound , 1998 .

[20]  Peter Vuust,et al.  Music in minor activates limbic structures: a relationship with dissonance? , 2008, Neuroreport.

[21]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Neonatal frequency discrimination in 250–4000-Hz range: Electrophysiological evidence , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[22]  K. Eklund,et al.  Event‐related brain potentials to change in rapidly presented acoustic stimuli in newborns , 1997 .

[23]  S. Trehub The developmental origins of musicality , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Maik C. Stüttgen,et al.  Computation of measures of effect size for neuroscience data sets , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[25]  K. Eklund,et al.  Maturational effects on newborn ERPs measured in the mismatch negativity paradigm , 2004, Experimental Neurology.

[26]  A. Friederici,et al.  Neural manifestation of cognitive and precognitive mismatch detection in early infancy , 2002, Neuroreport.

[27]  Gábor P. Háden,et al.  Newborn infants detect the beat in music , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Attentive novelty detection in humans is governed by pre-attentive sensory memory , 1994, Nature.

[29]  A. Izumi,et al.  Japanese monkeys perceive sensory consonance of chords. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[30]  Marc Hauser,et al.  Are consonant intervals music to their ears? Spontaneous acoustic preferences in a nonhuman primate , 2004, Cognition.

[31]  D. Perani,et al.  Functional specializations for music processing in the human newborn brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  Nobuo Masataka,et al.  Preference for consonance over dissonance by hearing newborns of deaf parents and of hearing parents. , 2006, Developmental science.

[33]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Musical training facilitates the neural discrimination of major versus minor chords in 13-year-old children. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[34]  L. Trainor,et al.  Musical experience, plasticity, and maturation: issues in measuring developmental change using EEG and MEG , 2012, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[35]  B. Schaal,et al.  Fetal sensory competencies. , 1996, European journal of obstetrics, gynecology, and reproductive biology.

[36]  M. Karjalainen,et al.  Short-term memory functions of the human fetus recorded with magnetoencephalography , 2005, Neuroreport.

[37]  R. G. Crowder,et al.  Perception of the major/minor distinction: V. Preferences among infants , 1991 .

[38]  S. Porges,et al.  Sleep state and vagal regulation of heart period patterns in the human newborn: an extension of the polyvagal theory. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[39]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  Preference for Sensory Consonance in 2- and 4-Month-Old Infants , 2002 .

[40]  R. Näätänen,et al.  The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[41]  Takako Fujioka,et al.  Neural Representation of Transposed Melody in Infants at 6 Months of Age , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Elmer S. West From the U. S. A. , 1965 .

[43]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Sensory consonance and the perceptual similarity of complex-tone harmonic intervals: tests of adult and infant listeners. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[44]  L. Trainor,et al.  Changes in auditory cortex and the development of mismatch negativity between 2 and 6 months of age. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[45]  I. Winkler,et al.  Newborn infants can organize the auditory world , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  T. F. Maher "Need for Resolution" Ratings for Harmonic Musical Intervals , 1976 .

[47]  Chao He,et al.  Mismatch Responses to Pitch Changes in Early Infancy , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  The preattentive processing of major vs. minor chords in the human brain: An event-related potential study , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[49]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Frequency discrimination at different frequency levels as indexed by electrophysiological and behavioral measures. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[50]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Newborn human brain identifies repeated auditory feature conjunctions of low sequential probability , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[51]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Chicks Like Consonant Music , 2011, Psychological science.

[52]  Gavin M. Bidelman,et al.  Neural Correlates of Consonance, Dissonance, and the Hierarchy of Musical Pitch in the Human Brainstem , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  Sarah J Wilson,et al.  Consonance and pitch. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[54]  J. Kagan,et al.  INFANTS' PERCEPTION OF CONSONANCE AND DISSONANCE IN MUSIC , 1998 .

[55]  R. Plomp,et al.  Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[56]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Event-related brain potential of human newborns to pitch change of an acoustic stimulus. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[57]  L. Trainor,et al.  Music acquisition: effects of enculturation and formal training on development , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Mismatch negativity indicates vowel discrimination in newborns , 1995, Hearing Research.

[59]  R. Näätänen,et al.  The newborn human brain binds sound features together , 2003, Neuroreport.

[60]  I. Peretz,et al.  Universal Recognition of Three Basic Emotions in Music , 2009, Current Biology.

[61]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Maturation of the auditory change detection response in infants: a longitudinal ERP study , 2002, Neuroreport.

[62]  R. G. Crowder,et al.  Perception of the Major/Minor Distinction: IV. Emotional Connotations in Young Children , 1990 .

[63]  L. Trainor,et al.  Melody recognition by two-month-old infants. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.