Do versus Don’t: The Impact of Framing on Goal-Level Setting

The consumer behavior literature extensively studied the impact of goal setting on behavior and performance. However, much less is known about the antecedents of goal level setting – consumers’ decision of whether to work out twice or three times per week. Consumers can decide how many goal-consistent activities to undertake (‘goal-consistent decision frame’; such as exercising two days per week) or to forego (‘goal-inconsistent decision frame’; such as not exercising five days per week). While objectively the same decision, we argue that these different frames impact consumers’ ambition. Making a decision to forego goal-consistent activities triggers negative, self-evaluative emotions and to compensate for these unfavorable self-evaluations, consumers set more ambitious goal levels. Across a variety of contexts, consumers are more ambitious when their focal decision is inconsistent with goal achievement. For instance, they decide to work out more often when they decide how many work-out sessions they would skip (versus attend). The impact of goal-inconsistent decision framing is mitigated when the activity is less instrumental towards goal achievement, and when negative self-evaluative emotions are alleviated through self-affirmation.

[1]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  The role of self-discrepancies in distinguishing regret from guilt , 2020, Self and Identity.

[2]  Szu‐chi Huang Social Information Avoidance: When, why, and how it is Costly in Goal Pursuit , 2018 .

[3]  Scott Wallace,et al.  How Goal Specificity Shapes Motivation: A Reference Points Perspective , 2018 .

[4]  On Amir,et al.  Guilt Dynamics: Consequences of Temporally Separating Decisions and Actions , 2017, Journal of Consumer Research.

[5]  Ying Zhang,et al.  Step by step: Sub-goals as a source of motivation , 2017 .

[6]  S. Shu,et al.  The Benefits of Emergency Reserves: Greater Preference and Persistence for Goals that Have Slack with a Cost , 2017 .

[7]  Jonathan Levav,et al.  The Compensatory Consumer Behavior Model: How Self-Discrepancies Drive Consumer Behavior , 2016 .

[8]  B. Monin,et al.  Consistency Versus Licensing Effects of Past Moral Behavior. , 2016, Annual review of psychology.

[9]  Rita Coelho do Vale,et al.  The benefits of behaving badly on occasion: Successful regulation by planned hedonic deviations☆ , 2016 .

[10]  K. White,et al.  Cross-Domain Effects of Guilt on Desire for Self-Improvement Products , 2015 .

[11]  A. Fishbach,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology It Was ( n ’ t ) Me : Exercising Restraint When Choices Appear Self-Diagnostic , 2015 .

[12]  Caleb Warren,et al.  The Progress Bias in Goal Pursuit: When One Step Forward Seems Larger than One Step Back , 2015 .

[13]  Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al.  The psychology of change: self-affirmation and social psychological intervention. , 2014, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  Ying Zhang,et al.  The unexpected positive impact of fixed structures on goal completion , 2013 .

[15]  S. Nowlis,et al.  The Effect of Goal Specificity on Consumer Goal Reengagement , 2013 .

[16]  David Gal,et al.  Can Small Victories Help Win the War? Evidence from Consumer Debt Management , 2012 .

[17]  Ying Zhang,et al.  So near and yet so far: the mental representation of goal progress. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  A. Fishbach,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: General the End Justifies the Means, but Only in the Middle , 2022 .

[19]  Ayelet Fishbach,et al.  Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 118 (2012) 99–107 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes , 2022 .

[20]  A. Mukhopadhyay,et al.  The Dynamics of Goal Revision: A Cybernetic Multiperiod Test-Operate-Test-Adjust-Loop (Total) Model of Self-Regulation , 2012 .

[21]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Prioritizing association strength versus value: the influence of self-regulatory modes on means evaluation in single goal and multigoal contexts. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Ying Zhang,et al.  Motivational Consequences of Perceived Velocity in Consumer Goal Pursuit , 2011 .

[23]  Keith Wilcox,et al.  Choice, Rejection, and Elaboration on Preference-Inconsistent Alternatives , 2011 .

[24]  Ying Zhang,et al.  Been There, Done That: The Impact of Effort Investment on Goal Value and Consumer Motivation , 2011 .

[25]  Minjung Koo,et al.  Climbing the Goal Ladder: How Upcoming Actions Increase Level of Aspiration Expressing Commitment versus Making Progress Remaining Actions Increase Level of Aspiration , 2022 .

[26]  Ying Zhang,et al.  The dynamics of self-regulation , 2009 .

[27]  Nira Liberman,et al.  Expectancy, value and psychological distance: A new look at goal gradients , 2008 .

[28]  A. Dijkstra,et al.  Self-evaluative emotions and expectations about self-evaluative emotions in health-behaviour change. , 2008, The British journal of social psychology.

[29]  Minjung Koo,et al.  Dynamics of self-regulation: How (un)accomplished goal actions affect motivation. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  R. Pieters,et al.  Goal-directed consumer behavior: Motivation, volition, and affect. , 2008 .

[31]  H. Klein,et al.  Goal choice and decision processes. , 2008 .

[32]  Gabriele Oettingen,et al.  Goal Setting and Goal Striving , 2007 .

[33]  Marcel Zeelenberg,et al.  Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt , 2007 .

[34]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Addressing Moderated Mediation Hypotheses: Theory, Methods, and Prescriptions , 2007, Multivariate behavioral research.

[35]  M. Biernat,et al.  Getting more from success: standard raising as esteem maintenance. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  Ran Kivetz,et al.  Determinants of justification and self-control. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  E. A. Locke,et al.  New Directions in Goal-Setting Theory , 2006 .

[38]  Ravi Dhar,et al.  Goals as Excuses or Guides: The Liberating Effect of Perceived Goal Progress on Choice , 2005 .

[39]  J. J. Donovan,et al.  Missing the mark: effects of time and causal attributions on goal revision in response to goal-performance discrepancies. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[40]  Alexander M. Czopp,et al.  Putting the brakes on prejudice: on the development and operation of cues for control. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. , 2002, The American psychologist.

[42]  Yaacov Schul,et al.  Inclusive and exclusive modes of thinking: Studies of prediction, preference, and social perception during parliamentary elections , 2002 .

[43]  M. Erez,et al.  Challenge versus threat effects on the goal–performance relationship , 2002 .

[44]  C. Whan Park,et al.  Choosing What I Want versus Rejecting What I Do Not Want: An Application of Decision Framing to Product Option Choice Decisions , 2000 .

[45]  R. Dhar,et al.  Consumer Choice between Hedonic and Utilitarian Goods , 2000 .

[46]  R. Bagozzi,et al.  Goal Setting and Goal Striving in Consumer Behavior , 1999 .

[47]  Schneider,et al.  All Frames Are Not Created Equal: A Typology and Critical Analysis of Framing Effects. , 1998, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[48]  Irwin P. Levin,et al.  Choosing versus rejecting options at different stages of decision making , 1998 .

[49]  Faten M. Moussa Determinants and Process of the Choice of Goal Difficulty , 1996 .

[50]  J. T. Austin,et al.  Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. , 1996 .

[51]  W. V. Eerde,et al.  Vroom's expectancy models and work-related criteria: A meta-analysis , 1996 .

[52]  Mark E. Tubbs,et al.  Expectancy, valence, and motivational force functions in goal-setting research: an empirical test , 1993 .

[53]  Vicki L. Goodwin,et al.  Meta-Analysis of the Antecedents of Personal Goal Level and of the Antecedents and Consequences of Goal Commitment , 1992 .

[54]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Self-regulation through goal setting , 1991 .

[55]  H. Klein Further evidence on the relationship between goal setting and expectancy theories. , 1991 .

[56]  Gregory B. Northcraft,et al.  Decision bias and personnel selection strategies , 1987 .

[57]  E. Higgins,et al.  Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. , 1987, Psychological review.

[58]  A. Bandura,et al.  Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems. , 1983 .

[59]  D. Campbell Determinants of choice of goal difficulty level: A review of situational and personality influences , 1982 .

[60]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Admission of Failure and Symbolic Self-Completion: Extending Lewinian Theory , 1982 .

[61]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Goal setting and task performance: 1969–1980. , 1981 .

[62]  J. W. Atkinson Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. , 1957, Psychological review.