Universal brain systems for recognizing word shapes and handwriting gestures during reading

Do the neural circuits for reading vary across culture? Reading of visually complex writing systems such as Chinese has been proposed to rely on areas outside the classical left-hemisphere network for alphabetic reading. Here, however, we show that, once potential confounds in cross-cultural comparisons are controlled for by presenting handwritten stimuli to both Chinese and French readers, the underlying network for visual word recognition may be more universal than previously suspected. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging in a semantic task with words written in cursive font, we demonstrate that two universal circuits, a shape recognition system (reading by eye) and a gesture recognition system (reading by hand), are similarly activated and show identical patterns of activation and repetition priming in the two language groups. These activations cover most of the brain regions previously associated with culture-specific tuning. Our results point to an extended reading network that invariably comprises the occipitotemporal visual word-form system, which is sensitive to well-formed static letter strings, and a distinct left premotor region, Exner’s area, which is sensitive to the forward or backward direction with which cursive letters are dynamically presented. These findings suggest that cultural effects in reading merely modulate a fixed set of invariant macroscopic brain circuits, depending on surface features of orthographies.

[1]  J. Tanji,et al.  Distinctions between dorsal and ventral premotor areas: anatomical connectivity and functional properties , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  M. Sigman,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .

[3]  L. Tan,et al.  Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture , 2004, Nature.

[4]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways , 2008, NeuroImage.

[5]  Roy Carr-Hill,et al.  International literacy statistics: a review of concepts, methodology and current data , 2008 .

[6]  A M Liberman,et al.  Perception of the speech code. , 1967, Psychological review.

[7]  A. H. Mack Developmental dyslexia in Chinese and English populations: dissociating the effect of dyslexia from language differences , 2011 .

[8]  M. Seghier,et al.  An anatomical signature for literacy , 2009, Nature.

[9]  R. Goebel,et al.  Integration of Letters and Speech Sounds in the Human Brain , 2004, Neuron.

[10]  L. Tan,et al.  Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  S. Dehaene,et al.  How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language , 2010, Science.

[12]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[13]  Shen-Hsing Annabel Chen,et al.  A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing , 2012, NeuroImage.

[14]  Randy L Buckner,et al.  Common and dissociable activation patterns associated with controlled semantic and phonological processing: evidence from FMRI adaptation. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Phonological Grammar Shapes the Auditory Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Task-specific change of unconscious neural priming in the cerebral language network , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. , 2000, Dyslexia.

[18]  G. Orban,et al.  The Retinotopic Organization of the Human Middle Temporal Area MT/V5 and Its Cortical Neighbors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Marcin Szwed,et al.  Unconsciously deciphering handwriting: Subliminal invariance for handwritten words in the visual word form area , 2010, NeuroImage.

[20]  H. Lyytinen,et al.  Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter–speech sound correspondences , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Kewei Chen,et al.  Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  M. Seghier,et al.  Developmental dyslexia in Chinese and English populations: dissociating the effect of dyslexia from language differences , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  S. Dehaene Reading in the Brain: The Science and Evolution of a Human Invention , 2009 .

[24]  J. Freyd,et al.  Representing the dynamics of a static form , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[25]  D Bavelier,et al.  Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: biological constraints and effects of experience. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  J. Xiong,et al.  Neural systems of second language reading are shaped by native language , 2003 .

[27]  E. Gentaz,et al.  The visuo-haptic and haptic exploration of letters increases the kindergarten-children's understanding of the alphabetic principle , 2004 .

[28]  Helen L Jamison,et al.  Morphology and the internal structure of words. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  A. Damasio,et al.  Troubled letters but not numbers. Domain specific cognitive impairments following focal damage in frontal cortex. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Jean-Luc Velay,et al.  Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing , 2003, NeuroImage.

[33]  W. Schneider,et al.  Cross‐cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[34]  P. Fox,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta‐analysis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[35]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cortical networks for vision and language in dyslexic and normal children of variable socio-economic status , 2012, NeuroImage.

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  Jean-François Démonet,et al.  The graphemic/motor frontal area Exner's area revisited , 2009, Annals of neurology.

[38]  P. Fox,et al.  The Neural System Underlying Chinese Logograph Reading , 2001, NeuroImage.

[39]  C. Price The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[40]  Brian A. Wandell,et al.  Visual Feature-Tolerance in the Reading Network , 2011, Neuron.

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[42]  M. Babcock,et al.  Perception of dynamic information in static handwritten forms. , 1988, The American journal of psychology.

[43]  Wai Ting Siok,et al.  Developmental dyslexia is characterized by the co-existence of visuospatial and phonological disorders in Chinese children , 2009, Current Biology.

[44]  Po-Lei Lee,et al.  Orthographic and phonological processing of Chinese characters: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[45]  J. Parkinson,et al.  Temporal Order of Strokes Primes letter Recognition , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[46]  L J Boë,et al.  Visual Perception of Motor Anticipation in Cursive Handwriting: Influence of Spatial and Movement Information on the Prediction of Forthcoming Letters , 1997, Perception.