The representation of periodic sounds in simulated sustained chopper units of the ventral cochlear nucleus.

The nature of the neural processing underlying the extraction of pitch information from harmonic complex sounds is still unclear. Electrophysiological studies in the auditory nerve and many psychophysical and modeling studies suggest that pitch might be extracted successfully by applying a mechanism like autocorrelation to the temporal discharge patterns of auditory-nerve fibers. The current modeling study investigates the possible role of populations of sustained chopper (Chop-S) units located in the mammalian ventral cochlear nucleus (VCN) in this process. First, it is shown that computer simulations can predict responses to periodic and quasiperiodic sounds of individual Chop-S units recorded in the guinea-pig VCN. Second, it is shown that the fundamental period of a periodic or quasiperiodic sound is represented in the first-order, interspike interval statistics of a population of simulated Chop-S units. This is true across a wide range of characteristic frequencies when the chopping rate is equal to the f0 of the sound. The model was able to simulate the results of psychophysical studies involving the pitch height and pitch strength of iterated ripple noise, the dominance region of pitch, the effect of phase on pitch height and pitch strength, pitch of inharmonic stimuli, and of sinusoidally amplitude modulated noise. Simulation results indicate that changes in the interspike interval statistics of populations of Chop-S units compare well with changes in the pitch perceived by humans. It is proposed that Chop-S units in the ventral cochlear nucleus may play an important role in pitch extraction: They can convert a purely temporal pitch code as observed in the auditory nerve into a temporal place code of pitch in populations of cochlear-nucleus, Chop-S with different characteristic frequencies, and chopping rates. Thus, populations of cochlear-nucleus Chop-S units, together with their target units presumably located in the inferior colliculus, may serve to establish a stable rate-place code of pitch at the level of the auditory cortex.

[1]  R. Patterson,et al.  A pulse ribbon model of monaural phase perception. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  R. Meddis,et al.  Implementation details of a computation model of the inner hair‐cell auditory‐nerve synapse , 1990 .

[3]  Ray Meddis,et al.  Adaptation in a revised inner-hair cell model. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  R. Meddis,et al.  A computer model of amplitude-modulation sensitivity of single units in the inferior colliculus. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  D. O. Kim,et al.  Amplitude-modulated tone encoding behavior of cochlear nucleus neurons: Modeling study , 1992, Hearing Research.

[6]  R Meddis,et al.  An evaluation of eight computer models of mammalian inner hair-cell function. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  Robert D Frisina,et al.  Encoding of amplitude modulation in the gerbil cochlear nucleus: I. A hierarchy of enhancement , 1990, Hearing Research.

[8]  J. Culling,et al.  Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking. II. The Fourcin pitch and the dichotic repetition pitch. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  Ronald J. MacGregor,et al.  Neural and brain modeling , 1987 .

[10]  E. Lopez-Poveda,et al.  A computational algorithm for computing nonlinear auditory frequency selectivity. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  A. Cheveigné Cancellation model of pitch perception. , 1998 .

[12]  Ray Meddis,et al.  A nonlinear filter-bank model of the guinea-pig cochlear nerve: rate responses. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  Ernst Terhardt,et al.  Calculating virtual pitch , 1979, Hearing Research.

[14]  E. Lopez-Poveda,et al.  A human nonlinear cochlear filterbank. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  R. Ritsma Frequencies dominant in the perception of the pitch of complex sounds. , 1966, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  W. Shofner,et al.  Temporal representation of rippled noise in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of the chinchilla. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  R Meddis,et al.  A computer model of a cochlear-nucleus stellate cell: responses to amplitude-modulated and pure-tone stimuli. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  B. Moore,et al.  Perception of the low pitch of frequency-shifted complexes. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  Ying-Cheng Lai,et al.  A model of selective processing of auditory-nerve inputs by stellate cells of the antero-ventral cochlear nucleus , 1994, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[20]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Neural correlates of the pitch of complex tones. II. Pitch shift, pitch ambiguity, phase invariance, pitch circularity, rate pitch, and the dominance region for pitch. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  W A Yost Pitch strength of iterated rippled noise when the pitch is ambiguous. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  R. Carlyon,et al.  The role of resolved and unresolved harmonics in pitch perception and frequency modulation discrimination. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  B. Moore,et al.  A Model of Loudness Applicable to Time-Varying Sounds , 2002 .

[24]  John F. Culling,et al.  Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking. , 1996 .

[25]  W. Shofner,et al.  Regularity and latency of units in ventral cochlear nucleus: implications for unit classification and generation of response properties. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  R. Patterson,et al.  The temporal representation of the delay of iterated rippled noise in the ventral cochlear nucleus of the guinea‐pig , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  Ray Meddis,et al.  Virtual pitch and phase sensitivity of a computer model of the auditory periphery , 1991 .

[28]  W. Yost Pitch strength of iterated rippled noise. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  I. Winter,et al.  Temporal representation of iterated rippled noise as a function of delay and sound level in the ventral cochlear nucleus. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  D. O. Kim,et al.  Responses of DCN-PVCN neurons and auditory nerve fibers in unanesthetized decerebrate cats to AM and pure tones: Analysis with autocorrelation/power-spectrum , 1990, Hearing Research.

[31]  P F Assmann,et al.  Pitches of concurrent vowels. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[32]  J. Licklider,et al.  A duplex theory of pitch perception , 1951, Experientia.

[33]  R. Meddis,et al.  Virtual pitch and phase sensitivity of a computer model of the auditory periphery. II: Phase sensitivity , 1991 .

[34]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Neural correlates of the pitch of complex tones. I. Pitch and pitch salience. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  M. Sachs,et al.  Classification of unit types in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus: PST histograms and regularity analysis. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  W. Yost Pitch of iterated rippled noise. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[37]  J. L. Goldstein An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[38]  Nace L. Golding,et al.  Synaptic inputs to stellate cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  A. de Cheveigné Cancellation model of pitch perception. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[40]  E. M. Burns,et al.  Played-again SAM: Further observations on the pitch of amplitude-modulated noise , 1981 .

[41]  M. Sachs,et al.  Vowel representations in the ventral cochlear nucleus of the cat: effects of level, background noise, and behavioral state. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  W. Shofner,et al.  Responses of cochlear nucleus units in the chinchilla to iterated rippled noises: analysis of neural autocorrelograms. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  J. Culling,et al.  Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking. I. Huggins' pitch and the "binaural edge pitch". , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[44]  R. Meddis Simulation of mechanical to neural transduction in the auditory receptor. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[45]  Ray Meddis,et al.  A revised model of the inner-hair cell and auditory-nerve complex. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[46]  S Shamma,et al.  The case of the missing pitch templates: how harmonic templates emerge in the early auditory system. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[47]  Robert D Frisina,et al.  Encoding of amplitude modulation in the gerbil cochlear nucleus: II. Possible neural mechanisms , 1990, Hearing Research.

[48]  J. W. Horst,et al.  Coding of spectral fine structure in the auditory nerve. I. Fourier analysis of period and interspike interval histograms. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[49]  J. Culling,et al.  Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking. III. The existence region of the Fourcin pitch. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[50]  Richard F. Lyon,et al.  Computational models of neural auditory processing , 1984, ICASSP.

[51]  J. Smurzyński,et al.  Pitch identification and discrimination for complex tones with many harmonics , 1990 .

[52]  M. Abeles Quantification, smoothing, and confidence limits for single-units' histograms , 1982, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.