Age-related differences in novelty and target processing among cognitively high performing adults

Previous research on age-related changes in ERP components in response to novel and target stimuli has not carefully controlled for differences in level of cognitive status between age groups, which may have contributed to the common findings of increased P3 latency, decreased P3 amplitude, and altered P3 scalp distribution. Here, cognitively high-performing (top third based on published norms) old, middle-aged, and young adults matched for IQ, education, and gender participated in a novelty oddball paradigm. There were no age-associated differences in P3 latency. Older adults had a larger, more anteriorly distributed P3 amplitude to all stimulus types, even repetitive standards, suggesting they may rely on increased resources and effortful frontal activity to successfully process any kind of visual stimulus. However, after controlling for this non-specific age-related processing difference, the amplitude and scalp distribution of the P3 component to novel and target stimuli were comparable across age groups, indicating that for cognitively high functioning elders there may be no age-related differences specific to the processing of novel and target events as indexed by the P3 component.

[1]  K. Walhovd,et al.  Two- and three-stimuli auditory oddball ERP tasks and neuropsychological measures in aging , 2001, Neuroreport.

[2]  Anders M. Fjell,et al.  P300 and Neuropsychological Tests as Measures of Aging: Scalp Topography and Cognitive Changes , 2004, Brain Topography.

[3]  R. Knight Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  D Friedman,et al.  Age-related changes in scalp topography to novel and target stimuli. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[5]  T W Picton,et al.  The effects of age on human event-related potentials. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[6]  M. Albert,et al.  Neurobiological Bases of Age-Related Cognitive Decline in the Rhesus Monkey , 1996, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[7]  R. Knight Distributed Cortical Network for Visual Attention , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  A. Kok Age-related changes in involuntary and voluntary attention as reflected in components of the event-related potential (ERP) , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[9]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[10]  J. Polich,et al.  Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[11]  E. Masliah,et al.  Quantitative synaptic alterations in the human neocortex during normal aging , 1993, Neurology.

[12]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Frontal and Parietal Components of a Cerebral Network Mediating Voluntary Attention to Novel Events , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  S. A. Hillyard,et al.  Changes in Visual Event-Related Potentials in Older Persons , 1979 .

[14]  J. Raven,et al.  Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .

[15]  I. Wickelgren The Aging Brain: For the Cortex, Neuron Loss May Be Less Than Thought , 1996, Science.

[16]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  J. Kounios,et al.  Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Age Differences in the Frontal Lateralization of Verbal and Spatial Working Memory Revealed by PET , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[20]  R T Knight,et al.  Age effects on the P300 to novel somatosensory stimuli. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  E Gordon,et al.  The effects of age on late components of the ERP and reaction time. , 1999, Experimental aging research.

[22]  E. Donchin,et al.  Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.

[23]  T. Tombaugh,et al.  The 60-item Boston Naming Test: norms for cognitively intact adults aged 25 to 88 years. , 1997, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[24]  H. Mayberg,et al.  Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson's disease. , 1992, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[25]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  ERP amplitude and scalp distribution to target and novel events: effects of temporal order in young, middle-aged and older adults. , 1994, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[26]  M. Fabiani,et al.  Changes in brain activity patterns in aging: the novelty oddball. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[27]  J. Raven Coloured progressive matrices : sets A, Ab, B , 1956 .

[28]  T. Crook,et al.  New adult age-and education-correction norms for the benton visual retention test. , 1993, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[29]  R. Sperling,et al.  Use of IQ-adjusted norms to predict progressive cognitive decline in highly intelligent older individuals. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[30]  P. Holcomb,et al.  The effects of aging on the P3 component of the visual event-related potential. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Neuropsychological tests' norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO , 1996 .

[32]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Assessing the development of automatic processing: An application of dual-task and event-related brain potential methodologies , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[33]  E. Donchin,et al.  On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[34]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  Clinical application of the P3 component of event-related potentials. I. Normal aging. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[35]  Kenneth R. Brizzee,et al.  Structural changes in the aging brain , 1981, Neurobiology of Aging.

[36]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Aging Gracefully: Compensatory Brain Activity in High-Performing Older Adults , 2002, NeuroImage.

[37]  R. Knight Aging decreases auditory event-related potentials to unexpected stimuli in humans , 1987, Neurobiology of Aging.

[38]  R Verleger,et al.  On the reasons for the delay of P3 latency in healthy elderly subjects. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[39]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Aging effects on auditory processing: an event-related potential study. , 1996, Experimental aging research.

[40]  M. Mesulam,et al.  The central role of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention to novel events. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  J. Snowdon,et al.  Validity of the Geriatric Depression Scale , 1990, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[42]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  Y. Stern What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[44]  Dc Washington Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. , 1994 .

[45]  J. Polich,et al.  EEG and ERP assessment of normal aging. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[46]  J. Rabe-Jabłońska,et al.  [Affective disorders in the fourth edition of the classification of mental disorders prepared by the American Psychiatric Association -- diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders]. , 1993, Psychiatria polska.

[47]  R. E. Dustman,et al.  Long latency components of the visually evoked potential in man: effects of aging. , 1980, Experimental aging research.

[48]  O. Spreen,et al.  A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .

[49]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[50]  D Friedman,et al.  An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[51]  J. Ford,et al.  Age-related changes in auditory event-related potentials. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  P. Satz Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. , 1993 .

[53]  Joseph J. Ryan,et al.  A screening procedure for estimating premorbid intelligence in the elderly , 1992 .

[54]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Toward a functional categorization of slow waves. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[55]  Nancy K. Squires,et al.  Comparison of endogenous event-related potentials in attend and non-attend conditions: latency changes with normal aging , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[56]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease , 1993, Neurology.

[57]  D Friedman,et al.  Effects of aging on the novelty P3 during attend and ignore oddball tasks. , 1998, Psychophysiology.