CHALLENGES AND POTENTIAL SOLUTIONS

FOR DECADES, DIGITAL INTEGRATED circuits (ICs) have benefited from the relentless progress predicted by Moore’s Law. With each new technology generation, ICs become denser, faster, and cheaper. However, with increasing system integration, as well as aggressive technology scaling, IC thermal issues now stand in the way of improved IC performance. Many of us may have watched a video from called What happens when the CPU cooler is removed? This two-minute video clip shows the effect of removing a heatsink from a running 1,400 MHz AMD Athlon processor: the whole system crashes, the CPU temperature shoots up to 698 ◦ F, and smoke starts coming out of the chip within a few seconds. Today’s microprocessors can get extremely hot. IC temperature is a strong function of IC power density. With each technology generation, the integration density of semiconductor devices doubles. As a result of reduction in both device feature size and supply voltage, the power consumption of each transistor decreases. However, increasing transistor counts and aggressive frequency scaling result in a significant increase in chip-power density, hence temperature. Increasing chip temperature has significant impact on other design metrics including reliability, performance, cost, and power consumption. Circuit reliability depends exponentially upon operating temperature. When temperature increases, major reliability issues, such as electromigration (which affects metal wires) and time-dependent dielectric breakdown (which affects transistors), become more important. Currently, temperature effects account for over 50% of electronic failures. IC temperature also affects circuit speed. Reduction of charge carrier mobility in

[1]  Konstantin K. Likharev,et al.  Single-electron devices and their applications , 1999, Proc. IEEE.

[2]  John L. Reeves,et al.  Memory for pain: Relation between past and present pain intensity , 1985, Pain.

[3]  J. Schmier,et al.  Patient recall and recall bias of health state and health status , 2004, Expert review of pharmacoeconomics & outcomes research.

[4]  George S. Howard,et al.  Response-shift bias: A source of contamination of self-report measures. , 1979 .

[5]  T. Dickfeld,et al.  Image integration in electroanatomic mapping , 2007, Herzschrittmachertherapie & Elektrophysiologie.

[6]  S. Naffziger,et al.  Power and temperature control on a 90-nm Itanium family processor , 2006, IEEE Journal of Solid-State Circuits.

[7]  S. Shiffman,et al.  Signaling does not adequately improve diary compliance , 2003, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[8]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Memories of colonoscopy: a randomized trial , 2003, Pain.

[9]  Philip M Clarke,et al.  Optimal recall length in survey design. , 2008, Journal of health economics.

[10]  A. Laurent,et al.  A summary of studies of interviewing methodology. , 1978, Vital and health statistics. Series 2, Data evaluation and methods research.

[11]  J. Zapka,et al.  Mammography use among sociodemographically diverse women: the accuracy of self-report. , 1996, American journal of public health.

[12]  A. E. Bergles,et al.  Evolution of cooling technology for electrical, electronic, and microelectronic equipment , 2003 .

[13]  P. H. Blaney Affect and memory: a review. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  Amanda C.C. Williams,et al.  Simple pain rating scales hide complex idiosyncratic meanings , 2000, Pain.

[15]  M. C. Reilly,et al.  The validity and accuracy of the Work Productivity and Activity Impairment questionnaire – irritable bowel syndrome version (WPAI:IBS) , 2004, Alimentary pharmacology & therapeutics.

[16]  C. Schwartz,et al.  Methodological approaches for assessing response shift in longitudinal health-related quality-of-life research. , 1999, Social science & medicine.

[17]  Floyd J. Fowler,et al.  Improving Survey Questions: Design and Evaluation , 1995 .

[18]  Ed Diener,et al.  Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions. , 1990 .

[19]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Patients' memories of painful medical treatments: real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures , 1996, Pain.

[20]  J. Pearson,et al.  Assessing the impact of chemotherapy-induced nausea and vomiting on patients' daily lives: a modified version of the Functional Living Index—Emesis (FLIE) with 5-day recall , 2003, Supportive Care in Cancer.

[21]  J. Bensing,et al.  Electronic diary assessment of pain, disability and psychological adaptation in patients differing in duration of pain , 2000, PAIN®.

[22]  P. Jones,et al.  American translation, modification, and validation of the St. George's Respiratory Questionnaire. , 2000, Clinical therapeutics.

[23]  W S Cash,et al.  Optimum recall period for reporting persons injured in motor vehicle accidents. , 1972, Vital and health statistics. Series 2, Data evaluation and methods research.

[24]  P Sandercock,et al.  Qualitative comparison of the reliability of health status assessments with the EuroQol and SF-36 questionnaires after stroke. United Kingdom Collaborators in the International Stroke Trial. , 1998, Stroke.

[25]  Madow Wg Interview data on chronic conditions compared with information derived from medical records. , 1967 .

[26]  A. Stone,et al.  Does the peak-end phenomenon observed in laboratory pain studies apply to real-world pain in rheumatoid arthritics? , 2000, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[27]  G. Slade,et al.  Effect of a 1-month vs. a 12-month reference period on responses to the 14-item Oral Health Impact Profile. , 2007, European journal of oral sciences.

[28]  N. Zethraeus,et al.  Femoral neck fractures in the elderly: Functional outcome and quality of life according to EuroQol , 2002, Quality of Life Research.

[29]  D. Kahneman,et al.  When More Pain Is Preferred to Less: Adding a Better End , 1993 .

[30]  A. Tellegen Structures of Mood and Personality and Their Relevance to Assessing Anxiety, With an Emphasis on Self-Report , 2019, Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders.

[31]  E. McColl Best practice in symptom assessment: a review , 2004, Gut.

[32]  H. Paulus,et al.  Dating the "window of therapeutic opportunity" in early rheumatoid arthritis: accuracy of patient recall of arthritis symptom onset. , 2004, The Journal of rheumatology.

[33]  T. Perneger,et al.  Prospective versus retrospective measurement of change in health status: a community based study in Geneva, Switzerland. , 1997, Journal of epidemiology and community health.

[34]  Khalid Sayood,et al.  Introduction to Data Compression , 1996 .

[35]  G. Grechi,et al.  COMPARISON OF HOSPITALIZATION REPORTING IN THREE SURVEY PROCEDURES. A STUDY OF ALTERNATIVE SURVEY METHODS FOR COLLECTION OF HOSPITALIZATION DATA FROM HOUSEHOLD RESPONDENTS. , 1965, Vital and health statistics. Ser. 1, Programs and collection procedures.

[36]  U BIERING-SORENSEN,et al.  REPORTING OF HOSPITALIZATION IN THE HEALTH INTERVIEW SURVEY. , 1965, Vital and health statistics. Ser. 1, Programs and collection procedures.

[37]  L. Rips,et al.  Answering autobiographical questions: the impact of memory and inference on surveys. , 1987, Science.

[38]  M. Litaker,et al.  Recency affects reporting accuracy of children's dietary recalls. , 2004, Annals of epidemiology.

[39]  J. Smyth,et al.  Ecological Momentary Assessment Research in Behavioral medicine , 2003 .

[40]  F. Roila,et al.  Impact of Nausea and Vomiting on Quality of Life in Cancer Patients During Chemotherapy , 2003, Health and quality of life outcomes.

[41]  A. Stone,et al.  Measuring clinical pain in chronic widespread pain: selected methodological issues. , 2003, Best practice & research. Clinical rheumatology.

[42]  M. Sprangers,et al.  Integrating response shift into health-related quality of life research: a theoretical model. , 1999, Social science & medicine.

[43]  A. Damiano,et al.  Comparison of responses to SF-36 Health Survey questions with one-week and four-week recall periods. , 1997, Health services research.

[44]  G. Harrison,et al.  Underreporting of food intake by dietary recall is not universal: a comparison of data from egyptian and american women. , 2000, The Journal of nutrition.

[45]  L. Rips,et al.  The Psychology of Survey Response , 2000 .

[46]  J. Hanley,et al.  Response shift influenced estimates of change in health-related quality of life poststroke. , 2004, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[47]  J. Cappelleri,et al.  Confirmatory factor analyses and reliability of the modified cigarette evaluation questionnaire. , 2007, Addictive behaviors.

[48]  Niraj K. Jha,et al.  Power-Aware Architectural Synthesis , 2007 .

[49]  Joseph E. Schwartz,et al.  Variability of Momentary Pain Predicts Recall of Weekly Pain: A Consequence of the Peak (or Salience) Memory Heuristic , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[50]  J. Broderick,et al.  The association of depression and neuroticism with pain reports: a comparison of momentary and recalled pain assessment. , 2007, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[51]  Balaram Sinharoy,et al.  Design and implementation of the POWER5 microprocessor , 2004, Proceedings. 41st Design Automation Conference, 2004..

[52]  Charles P. Thompson,et al.  Social memory in everyday life: Recall of self-events and other-events. , 1991 .

[53]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[54]  Saul Shiffman,et al.  Remember that? A comparison of real-time versus retrospective recall of smoking lapses. , 1997, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[55]  C. Sledge,et al.  Pitfalls of Using Patient Recall to Derive Preoperative Status in Outcome Studies of Total Knee Arthroplasty , 2001, The Journal of bone and joint surgery. American volume.

[56]  Seymour Sudman,et al.  Contributions of cognitive psychology to advertising research. , 1989 .

[57]  George S. Howard,et al.  Response-Shift Bias , 1980 .

[58]  Dennis Gannon,et al.  A survey of data provenance techniques , 2005 .

[59]  S. Shiffman,et al.  Patient non-compliance with paper diaries , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[60]  P. Totterdell,et al.  Time Frames for Mood: Relations between Momentary and Generalized Ratings of Affect , 1995 .

[61]  S. Shiffman,et al.  Understanding recall of weekly pain from a momentary assessment perspective: absolute agreement, between- and within-person consistency, and judged change in weekly pain , 2004, Pain.

[62]  R. Delamarter,et al.  Low Back Pain Recollection Versus Concurrent Accounts: Outcomes Analysis , 2002, Spine.

[63]  Barbara A. Gutek,et al.  On the Accuracy of Retrospective Attitudinal Data , 1978 .

[64]  John Bound,et al.  Measurement error in survey data , 2001 .

[65]  James F. Brinkley,et al.  Issues in biomedical research data management and analysis: needs and barriers. , 2007, Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association : JAMIA.

[66]  S. Shariat,et al.  Reliability of remembered International Index of Erectile Function domain scores in men with localized prostate cancer. , 2005, Urology.

[67]  Joseph E. Schwartz,et al.  Intensive momentary reporting of pain with an electronic diary: reactivity, compliance, and patient satisfaction , 2003, Pain.

[68]  Blaz Zupan,et al.  Predictive data mining in clinical medicine: Current issues and guidelines , 2008, Int. J. Medical Informatics.

[69]  M. Litwin,et al.  Accuracy of recall in health-related quality-of-life assessment among men treated for prostate cancer. , 1999, Journal of clinical oncology : official journal of the American Society of Clinical Oncology.

[70]  David A. Williams,et al.  Pain Assessment in Patients With Fibromyalgia Syndrome: A Consideration of Methods for Clinical Trials , 2004, The Clinical journal of pain.