Writing About Testing Worries Boosts Exam Performance in the Classroom

Write Your Worries Away Tests and exams are stressful for many people. Students who “choke” at an exam may perform less well than their knowledge base warrants. Such results can accumulate to generate reduced educational achievements and expectations. Studying young adults performing math tests, Ramirez and Beilock (p. 211) found that a brief intervention—writing about their anxiety about the upcoming exam—helped students to do better in the exam. Perhaps by acknowledging their fears, students were able to tame distracting emotions. A brief classroom intervention helps remove anxiety from the testing situation. Two laboratory and two randomized field experiments tested a psychological intervention designed to improve students’ scores on high-stakes exams and to increase our understanding of why pressure-filled exam situations undermine some students’ performance. We expected that sitting for an important exam leads to worries about the situation and its consequences that undermine test performance. We tested whether having students write down their thoughts about an upcoming test could improve test performance. The intervention, a brief expressive writing assignment that occurred immediately before taking an important test, significantly improved students’ exam scores, especially for students habitually anxious about test taking. Simply writing about one’s worries before a high-stakes exam can boost test scores.

[1]  Anne Maass,et al.  Why Do Women Underperform Under Stereotype Threat? , 2005, Psychological science.

[2]  L. Hardy,et al.  Knowledge and conscious control of motor actions under stress. , 1996, British journal of psychology.

[3]  R. Baumeister Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Valerie Purdie-Vaughns,et al.  Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation: Intervening to Close the Minority Achievement Gap , 2009, Science.

[5]  D M Wegner,et al.  Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  K. Klein,et al.  Expressive writing can increase working memory capacity. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Choke: What the Secrets of the Brain Reveal About Getting It Right When You Have To , 2010 .

[8]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  When High-Powered People Fail , 2005, Psychological science.

[9]  Michael Johns,et al.  Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Math Performance in Stressful Situations , 2008 .

[11]  Jerrell C. Cassady,et al.  Cognitive Test Anxiety and Academic Performance , 2002 .

[12]  Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al.  Reducing the Racial Achievement Gap: A Social-Psychological Intervention , 2006, Science.

[13]  J. Smyth,et al.  Written emotional expression: effect sizes, outcome types, and moderating variables. , 1998, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[14]  Marci S. DeCaro,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Diagnosing and Alleviating the Impact of Performance Pressure on Mathematical Problem Solving , 2022 .

[15]  A. Miyake,et al.  Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control , 1999 .

[16]  Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al.  Supporting Online Material for Reducing the Gender Achievement Gap in College Science: A Classroom Study of Values Affirmation , 2010 .

[17]  Chris S. Hulleman,et al.  Promoting Interest and Performance in High School Science Classes , 2009, Science.

[18]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  More on the fragility of performance: choking under pressure in mathematical problem solving. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.