Language Skills of Children with Early Cochlear Implantation

Objective This study investigated factors contributing to the comprehension and production of English language by children with prelingual deafness after 4 to 7 yr of multichannel cochlear implant use. The analysis controlled for the effects of child and family characteristics so that educational factors most conducive to maximum implant benefit could be identified. Design A battery of language tests were administered to 181 8- and 9 - yr-old children from across the United States and Canada who received a cochlear implant by age 5. Tests of comprehension, verbal reasoning, narrative ability and spontaneous language production were administered either in speech and sign or in the child’s preferred communication mode. These constituted the Total Language measures. Spoken Language measures were derived from a speech-only language sample. Type and amount of educational intervention since implantation constituted the independent variables. Characteristics of the child and the family were considered intervening variables. A series of multiple regression analyses determined the amount of variance in Total Language and Spoken Language ability accounted for by the intervening variables and the amount of additional variance attributable to the independent variables. Results More than half of the children (with performance intelligence quotients in the average range) exhibited language skills that were similar to those of hearing 8 to 9 yr olds on measures of verbal reasoning, narrative ability, utterance length, and lexical diversity. Significant predictors of language ability were similar for Total and for Spoken Language outcomes and included greater nonverbal intelligence, smaller family size, higher socio-economic status and female gender. Age at receiving an implant did not affect language outcome. After the variance due to these variables was controlled, the primary rehabilitative factors associated with linguistic outcome were amount of mainstream class placement and an educational emphasis on speech and auditory skills. Conclusions Use of a cochlear implant has had a dramatic impact on the linguistic competence of profoundly hearing-impaired children. More than half of the children in this sample with average learning ability produced and understood English language at a level comparable with that of their hearing age mates. Such mature language outcomes were not typical of children with profound hearing loss who used hearing aids. Use of a visual (i.e., sign) language system did not provide the linguistic advantage that had been anticipated. Children educated without use of sign exhibited a significant advantage in their use of narratives, the breadth of their vocabulary, in their use of bound morphemes, in the length of their utterances and in the complexity of the syntax used in their spontaneous language. An oral educational focus provided a significant advantage for both spoken and total language skills.

[1]  A. Geers,et al.  Factors Predictive of the Development of Literacy in Profoundly Hearing-Impaired Adolescents. , 1989 .

[2]  A. Geers,et al.  Spoken Language Results: Vocabulary, Syntax, and Communication. , 1994 .

[3]  A. Geers,et al.  Syntactic maturity of spontaneous speech and elicited imitations of hearing-impaired children. , 1978, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[4]  A. Boothroyd,et al.  Practical Implications of Cochlear Implants in Children , 1991, Ear and hearing.

[5]  J. Tomblin,et al.  A comparison of language achievement in children with cochlear implants and children using hearing aids. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[6]  S. Warschausky,et al.  WISC-III index growth curve characteristics following traumatic brain injury. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children--Third Edition. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[7]  A. Geers,et al.  Background and Educational Characteristics of Prelingually Deaf Children Implanted by Five Years of Age , 2003, Ear and hearing.

[8]  M. Lutman,et al.  Comparison of Early Communicative Behavior in Young Children with Cochlear Implants and with Hearing Aids , 1994, Ear and hearing.

[9]  Rudolph F. Martin,et al.  Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language , 1977 .

[10]  A E Geers,et al.  Predicting long-term benefits from single-channel cochlear implants in profoundly hearing-impaired children. , 1988, The American journal of otology.

[11]  M. Lutman,et al.  The Predictive Value of Measures of Preverbal Communicative Behaviors in Young Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants , 1997, Ear and hearing.

[12]  Maureen D. Haynes,et al.  Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language–Revised (TACL–R) , 1989, Diagnostique.

[13]  David B. Pisoni,et al.  Language Development in Profoundly Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants , 2000, Psychological science.

[14]  Lisa Davidson,et al.  Factors Associated with Development of Speech Perception Skills in Children Implanted by Age Five , 2003, Ear and hearing.

[15]  A. Geers,et al.  Speech and language acquisition in young children after cochlear implantation. , 1999, Otolaryngologic clinics of North America.

[16]  A. Geers,et al.  Analysis of Narrative Ability in Children with Cochlear Implants , 2001, Ear and hearing.

[17]  A E Geers,et al.  Speech perception and production skills of students with impaired hearing from oral and total communication education settings. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[18]  A. Geers,et al.  Use of Speech by Children From Total Communication Programs Who Wear Cochlear Implants , 2002 .

[19]  K. W. Hunt Grammatical structures written at three grade levels , 1965 .

[20]  M. Svirsky,et al.  Children with implants can speak, but can they communicate? , 1997, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[21]  H. Scarborough Index of Productive Syntax , 1990, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[22]  A. Geers,et al.  Acquisition of spoken and signed English by profoundly deaf children. , 1984, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[23]  P J Blamey,et al.  Relationships among speech perception, production, language, hearing loss, and age in children with impaired hearing. , 2001, Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research.

[24]  R. Miyamoto,et al.  Effects of Age at Implantation in Young Children , 2002, The Annals of otology, rhinology & laryngology. Supplement.

[25]  Janet E. Green,et al.  Language Development in Children Implanted with the Clarion® Cochlear Implant , 1999, The Annals of otology, rhinology & laryngology. Supplement.

[26]  T. Zwolan,et al.  Speech, vocabulary, and the education of children using cochlear implants: oral or total communication? , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[27]  E. J. Barker,et al.  A Clinical Report on Receptive Vocabulary Skills in Cochlear Implant Users , 1995, Ear and hearing.