Unintentional force changes in cyclical tasks performed by an abundant system: Empirical observations and a dynamical model

The study explored unintentional force changes elicited by removing visual feedback during cyclical, two-finger isometric force production tasks. Subjects performed two types of tasks at 1Hz, paced by an auditory metronome. One - Force task - required cyclical changes in total force while maintaining the sharing, defined as relative contribution of a finger to total force. The other task - Share task - required cyclical changes in sharing while keeping total force unchanged. Each trial started under full visual feedback on both force and sharing; subsequently, feedback on the variable that was instructed to stay constant was frozen, and finally feedback on the other variable was also removed. In both tasks, turning off visual feedback on total force elicited a drop in the mid-point of the force cycle and an increase in the peak-to-peak force amplitude. Turning off visual feedback on sharing led to a drift of mean share toward 50:50 across both tasks. Without visual feedback there was consistent deviation of the two force time series from the in-phase pattern (typical of the Force task) and from the out-of-phase pattern (typical of the Share task). This finding is in contrast to most earlier studies that demonstrated only two stable patterns, in-phase and out-of-phase. We interpret the results as consequences of drifts of parameters in a dynamical system leading in particular to drifts in the referent finger coordinates toward their actual coordinates. The relative phase desynchronization is caused by the right-left differences in the hypothesized drift processes, consistent with the dynamic dominance hypothesis.

[1]  M. Latash,et al.  The nature of constant and cyclic force production: unintentional force-drift characteristics , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  M. Latash,et al.  Intentional and unintentional multi-joint movements: Their nature and structure of variance , 2015, Neuroscience.

[3]  D. Vaillancourt,et al.  Neural Basis for the Processes That Underlie Visually-guided and Internally-guided Force Control in Humans , 2003 .

[4]  Charley W. Lafe,et al.  Adapting relative phase of bimanual isometric force coordination through scaling visual information intermittency. , 2016, Human movement science.

[5]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[6]  David E. Vaillancourt,et al.  Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Michel J. H. Heijnen,et al.  Factors leading to obstacle contact during adaptive locomotion , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Mark L Latash,et al.  On the nature of unintentional action: a study of force/moment drifts during multifinger tasks. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky,et al.  Do synergies decrease force variability? A study of single-finger and multi-finger force production , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Internal vs external generation of movements: differential neural pathways involved in bimanual coordination performed in the presence or absence of augmented visual feedback , 2003, NeuroImage.

[11]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  The dynamics of isometric bimanual coordination , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Mark L Latash,et al.  Equifinality and its violations in a redundant system: control with referent configurations in a multi-joint positional task. , 2014, Motor control.

[14]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.

[15]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy) , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[17]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Michel J. H. Heijnen,et al.  Memory-guided obstacle crossing: more failures were observed for the trail limb versus lead limb , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Coordination of upper and lower limb segments: deficits on the ipsilesional side after unilateral stroke , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  V. M. Zatsiorsky,et al.  Synergies in the space of control variables within the equilibrium-point hypothesis , 2016, Neuroscience.

[21]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[22]  Andrew D. Wilson,et al.  Learning a coordinated rhythmic movement with task-appropriate coordination feedback , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Finger force changes in the absence of visual feedback in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2016, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  Michael Stöckl,et al.  On the use of continuous relative phase: Review of current approaches and outline for a new standard. , 2014, Clinical biomechanics.

[25]  Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky,et al.  Factors affecting grip force: anatomy, mechanics, and referent configurations , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J. Summers,et al.  The Dynamics of Bimanual Circle Drawing , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[27]  Marcos Duarte,et al.  Do synergies improve accuracy? A study of speed-accuracy trade-offs during finger force production. , 2008, Motor control.

[28]  M. Inase,et al.  Neuronal activity in the primate premotor, supplementary, and precentral motor cortex during visually guided and internally determined sequential movements. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Anatol G. Feldman,et al.  Threshold position control of arm movement with anticipatory increase in grip force , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  The detuning factor in the dynamics of interlimb rhythmic coordination , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[32]  David E Vaillancourt,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of brain activity during the transition from visually guided to memory-guided force control. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Dynamics of 1:2 Coordination: Sources of Symmetry Breaking. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  K. Newell,et al.  Intermittency in the control of continuous force production. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  G E Loeb,et al.  What might the brain know about muscles, limbs and spinal circuits? , 1999, Progress in brain research.

[36]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  The learning of 90° continuous relative phase with and without Lissajous feedback: external and internally generated bimanual coordination. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[37]  James C Craig,et al.  Proprioceptive perception of phase variability. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control: Defining Central Concepts , 2015 .

[39]  Mark L Latash,et al.  Motor synergies and the equilibrium-point hypothesis. , 2010, Motor control.

[40]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Bimanual coordination and the intermittency of visual information in isometric force tracking , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky,et al.  Processes underlying unintentional finger-force changes in the absence of visual feedback , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  M. Latash Virtual trajectories, joint stiffness, and changes in the limb natural frequency during single-joint oscillatory movements , 1992, Neuroscience.

[44]  J. Kelso,et al.  Action-Perception as a Pattern Formation Process , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[45]  D E Sherwood,et al.  Hand preference, practice order, and spatial assimilations in rapid bimanual movement. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[46]  M. Latash,et al.  Optimality and stability of intentional and unintentional actions: I. Origins of drifts in performance , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  P. Goldman-Rakic Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.

[48]  Benoît G. Bardy,et al.  Beyond in-phase and anti-phase coordination in a model of joint action , 2015, Biological Cybernetics.

[49]  Andrew D. Wilson,et al.  Identifying the information for the visual perception of relative phase , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[50]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Redundancy, Self-Motion, and Motor Control , 2009, Neural Computation.

[51]  K. Newell,et al.  Visual control of isometric force in Parkinson's disease , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Mark L Latash,et al.  Biological Movement and Laws of Physics. , 2017, Motor control.

[53]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Symmetrical and asymmetrical influences on force production in 1:2 and 2:1 bimanual force coordination tasks , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  J. Buchanan,et al.  Amplitude Scaling in a Bimanual Circle-Drawing Task: Pattern Switching and End-Effector Variability , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[55]  Alan M. Wilson,et al.  Gait characterisation and classification in horses , 2007, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[56]  M. Latash,et al.  Changes in multifinger interaction and coordination in Parkinson's disease. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  M. Latash,et al.  Task-Specific Stability of Multifinger Steady-State Action , 2015, Journal of motor behavior.

[58]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Handedness: Differential Specializations for Control of Trajectory and Position , 2005, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[59]  Joshua L. Haworth,et al.  Coordination dynamics of (a)symmetrically loaded gait , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  J A Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. , 1988, Science.

[61]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Toward a new theory of motor synergies. , 2007, Motor control.

[62]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Towards physics of neural processes and behavior , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[63]  Sasha Reschechtko,et al.  Stability of multifinger action in different state spaces. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  Mark L Latash,et al.  Equifinality and its violations in a redundant system: multifinger accurate force production. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  H. Simon,et al.  Rational choice and the structure of the environment. , 1956, Psychological review.

[66]  R. Sainburg Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  M. Latash,et al.  Force sharing among fingers as a model of the redundancy problem , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.