The prospects for perfect vision.

R efractive surgery and other high-tech methods for correcting the optical aberrations of the eye aim to make the eye optically perfect. The notion that perfect vision may be within our grasp is a distinctly 21st century concept which is a quantum leap forward from the foundations of oph-thalmic practice in the 20th century. Throughout history the assumption has been that the chief cause of poor vision is poor optical quality of the retinal image. From this basic principle, the following line of argument leads one to contemplate the prospects for perfect vision through refractive surgery or other means. Refractive errors are common , at least in the developed countries of the world, and these uncorrected refractive errors cause the retinal image to become blurred. This uncor-rected optical blur reduces visual performance on almost every visual task imaginable because it reduces contrast in the retinal image. The successful history of ophthalmic clinical practice in the 20th century is proof that elimination of sphero-cylindrical blur with spectacles, contact lenses, intraocular lenses, and refractive surgery is a miracle cure that literally restores sight to the blind. Therefore, it stands to reason that the 21st century goal of eliminating all traces of optical blur due to higher-order aberrations of the eye, which clinicians sometimes call "irregular astigmatism," suggests the prospect of perfect retinal images producing perfect vision. If the goal is to achieve retinal images of such high quality that they are no longer limited by the optical imperfections of the eye, then the only remaining optical limitation will be the unavoidable effects of diffraction. This is the meaning of the phrase "diffraction-limited retinal images." When contemplating the prospects for achieving diffrac-tion-limited retinal images even under night-time viewing conditions when pupils are dilated, three questions immediately come to mind. 1) Is this quest for diffraction-limited image quality really possible, or is it just wishful thinking? 2) What would be the potential benefits of perfect retinal images, if they can be obtained? 3) What are the potential penalties? In other words, is there a down side to having perfect retinal images? The purpose of this brief article is to frame these questions in terms that are understandable to clini-cians and visual scientists alike, and at the same time to expose some of the complexity of the issues raised. ARE PERFECT RETINAL IMAGES REALLY POSSIBLE? Other papers in this feature issue address the technical issues …

[1]  L N Thibos,et al.  Statistical distribution of foveal transverse chromatic aberration, pupil centration, and angle psi in a population of young adult eyes. , 1995, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[2]  R A Applegate,et al.  Changes in corneal wavefront aberrations with aging. , 1999, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[3]  D. Williams,et al.  Cone spacing and the visual resolution limit. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[4]  L. N. Thibos,et al.  Vision beyond the resolution limit: Aliasing in the periphery , 1987, Vision Research.

[5]  L N Piotrowski,et al.  A Demonstration of the Visual Importance and Flexibility of Spatial-Frequency Amplitude and Phase , 1982, Perception.

[6]  A. Bradley,et al.  Spherical Aberration of the Reduced Schematic Eye with Elliptical Refracting Surface , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[7]  David R. Williams,et al.  Consequences of spatial sampling for human motion perception , 1990, Vision Research.

[8]  David A. Atchison,et al.  Measurement of monochromatic ocular aberrations of human eyes as a function of accommodation by the howland aberroscope technique , 1995, Vision Research.

[9]  David Williams Topography of the foveal cone mosaic in the living human eye , 1988, Vision Research.

[10]  L N Thibos,et al.  Sampling limits and critical bandwidth for letter discrimination in peripheral vision. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[11]  A. Bradley,et al.  Characterization of spatial aliasing and contrast sensitivity in peripheral vision , 1996, Vision Research.

[12]  D. Williams,et al.  Consequences of spatial sampling by a human photoreceptor mosaic. , 1983, Science.

[13]  A Bradley,et al.  Effects of refractive error on detection acuity and resolution acuity in peripheral vision. , 1997, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[14]  A. Hendrickson,et al.  Human photoreceptor topography , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[15]  Stephen J. Anderson,et al.  Post-receptoral undersampling in normal human peripheral vision , 1990, Vision Research.

[16]  David R. Williams,et al.  Off-axis optical quality and retinal sampling in the human eye , 1996, Vision Research.

[17]  L N Thibos,et al.  Clinical applications of the Shack-Hartmann aberrometer. , 1999, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[18]  A. Bradley,et al.  The chromatic eye: a new reduced-eye model of ocular chromatic aberration in humans. , 1992, Applied optics.

[19]  D. Williams,et al.  Visibility of interference fringes near the resolution limit. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[20]  D. G. Green,et al.  Optical and retinal factors affecting visual resolution. , 1965, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  D R Williams,et al.  Supernormal vision and high-resolution retinal imaging through adaptive optics. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.