The neuropsychology of visual artistic production

What happens to visual artists with neuropsychological deficits? This review will examine artistic production in individuals with a variety of syndromes including achromatopsia, neglect, visual agnosia, aphasia, epilepsy, migraine, dementia and autism. From this review it appears that artists are not spared visual-motor deficits despite their special graphic abilities. Rather their talents allow them to express visual deficits with particular eloquence. By contrast, the effects of aphasia on art are variable. In addition to deficits, neuropsychological syndromes may be associated with positive phenomena. Such phenomena induced by epilepsy or migraines can serve to inspire artists. This review also makes clear that artists with neuropsychological deficits do not necessarily produce art of lesser quality. Rather, their art may change in content or in style, sometimes in surprising and aesthetically pleasing ways. The neuropsychology of visual art also touches on a few central questions about the nature of artistic expression itself. For example, what forms can artistic representations take? How are visual features used descriptively and expressively? What roles do knowing and seeing play in depiction?

[1]  Wendy Heller,et al.  Cognitive and Emotional Organization of the Brain: Influences on the Creation and Perception of Art , 1994 .

[2]  G. Fuller,et al.  Migraine aura as artistic inspiration. , 1988, BMJ.

[3]  A Chatterjee,et al.  Cortical blindness and visual imagery , 1995, Neurology.

[4]  B. Hermelin,et al.  Visual and motor functions in graphically gifted savants , 1994, Psychological Medicine.

[5]  P. Gorelick,et al.  Personality changes in Alzheimer's disease. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[6]  Z. Bujas-Petković [Special talents of autistic children (autistic-savant) and their mental functions]. , 1994, Liječnički vjesnik.

[7]  D. Benson,et al.  Effects of a right-hemisphere stroke on an artist's performance , 1993 .

[8]  H. Lissauer,et al.  Ein Fall von Seelenblindheit nebst einem Beitrage zur Theorie derselben , 1890, Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten.

[9]  R. Schultz,et al.  The Science of ART , 2002, Science.

[10]  J. Marshall,et al.  The art of visual neglect , 1997, The Lancet.

[11]  M. Botez,et al.  Defective Revisualization: Dissociation Between Cognitive and Imagistic Thought Case Report and Short Review of the Literature , 1985, Cortex.

[12]  J. Marshall,et al.  Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect , 1988, Nature.

[13]  V. Lewis,et al.  Skill, content and generative strategies in autistic children's drawings , 1991 .

[14]  O. Blanke,et al.  Colour neglect in an artist , 2003, The Lancet.

[15]  Orhan Pamuk,et al.  My Name Is Red , 1998 .

[16]  M Wilkinson,et al.  Migraine Art , 1985, Cephalalgia : an international journal of headache.

[17]  L. Waterhouse,et al.  Brief report: Fragmented drawings in autistic children , 1990 .

[18]  Jason W. Brown Mind, brain, and consciousness : the neuropsychology of cognition , 1977 .

[19]  K. Podoll,et al.  Migraine experiences as artistic inspiration in a contemporary artist , 2000, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[20]  T ALAJOUANINE,et al.  Aphasia and artistic realization. , 1948, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Howard Gardner The shattered mind: The person after brain damage , 1975 .

[22]  E. Gombrich Art and Illusion , 1962 .

[23]  Jason W. Brown Mind, brain, and consciousness : the neuropsychology of cognition , 1977 .

[24]  B L Miller,et al.  Emergence of artistic talent in frontotemporal dementia , 1998, Neurology.

[25]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience , 1999 .

[26]  N. O’connor,et al.  Talents and preoccupations in idiots-savants , 1991, Psychological Medicine.

[27]  H. Coslett Neglect in vision and visual imagery: a double dissociation. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Break-Down of Perceptual Awareness in Unilateral Neglect , 1990, Cortex.

[29]  Anjan Chatterjee,et al.  Prospects for a cognitive neuroscience of visual aesthetics , 2003 .

[30]  J. Marshall,et al.  Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  P. Sommers A system for drawing and drawing-related neuropsychology , 1989 .

[32]  H. Gardner,et al.  Visual Agnosia in an Artist , 1978, Cortex.

[33]  B. Kaczmarek,et al.  Aphasia in an artist: A disorder of symbolic processing , 1991 .

[34]  Myrna F. Schwartz,et al.  Modular deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia , 1990 .

[35]  S. Zeki,et al.  Inner Vision: An Exploration of Art and the Brain , 2000 .

[36]  A Chatterjee,et al.  Picturing unilateral spatial neglect: viewer versus object centred reference frames. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[37]  Semir Zeki,et al.  Art and the brain , 1998 .

[38]  A. Cantagallo,et al.  Preserved Insight in an Artist with Extrapersonal Spatial Neglect , 1998, Cortex.

[39]  L. Selfe,et al.  Nadia: A Case of Extraordinary Drawing Ability in an Autistic Child , 1978 .

[40]  N. Geschwind,et al.  The interictal behavior syndrome of temporal lobe epilepsy. , 1975, Archives of general psychiatry.

[41]  G. G. Marsh,et al.  Unilateral Neglect and Constructional Apraxia in a Right-Handed Artist With A Left Posterior Lesion , 1987, Cortex.

[42]  J. Bennett Vision and Art: The Biology of Seeing , 2003 .

[43]  M Behrmann,et al.  Intact visual imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient with visual agnosia. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  R. Rafal Neglect , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.