Perceptual Consequences of Conductive Hearing Loss: Speech Perception in Indigenous Students Learning English as a 'School' Language

The high incidence of ear disease and hearing loss in Australian Indigenous children is well documented. This study aims to consider the effect of hearing loss and native-language phonology on learning English by Australian Indigenous children. Twenty-one standard Australian English consonants were considered in a consonant-vowel (CV) context. Each consonant was paired with each other to yield 'same' and 'different' consonant pairs. The participants were classified into three groups: (1) English speaking, non- Indigenous children without history of hearing loss and otitis media (three males, four females, mean age 13.7 years); (2) Indigenous children speaking Tiwi as their native language, without history of hearing loss and otitis media and learning English as a second language (two males, three females, mean age 12.1 yrs) and (3) Indigenous children speaking Tiwi as their native language, with a history of hearing loss and otitis media since childhood (six females, mean age 13.1 years). The reaction time from the onset of the second word of the pair to the pressing of a 'same' or 'different' button was measured. The results demonstrated that discrimination of consonants was differentially affected by differences in language. Hearing loss further complicated the difficulties that a child was already having with English. Hearing loss tended to affect discrimination of English consonants more than those in the native language. The study suggests that amplification alone does not suffice and recommended that phonological awareness programs, with or without amplification, need to be part of a reading program from preschool with Indigenous children learning English as a 'school' language.

[1]  W. T. McCoy,et al.  RESPIRATORY INFECTIONS IN AUSTRALIAN ABORIGINAL CHILDREN: A CLINICAL AND RADIOLOGICAL STUDY , 1968, The Medical journal of Australia.

[2]  J D Chananie,et al.  Choice response time and distinctive features in speech discrimination. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  David Kerry Kirke,et al.  Aboriginal infant and toddler mortality and morbidity in Central Australia, 1965-1969 , 1970 .

[4]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Identification versus Discrimination of Distinctive Features in Speech Perception , 1972, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  S Singh,et al.  A comparison of four feature systems using data from three psychophysical methods. , 1972, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[6]  M. D. Wang,et al.  Consonant confusions in noise: a study of perceptual features. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  F F Weiner,et al.  Multidimensional analysis of choice reaction time judgments on pairs of English fricatives. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  N Lewis,et al.  Otitis media and linguistic incompetence. , 1976, Archives of otolaryngology.

[9]  W D Mustain Linguistic and educational implications of recurrent otitis media. , 1979, Ear, nose, & throat journal.

[10]  Robert A. Dobie,et al.  Influence of Otitis Media on Hearing and Development , 1979, The Annals of otology, rhinology & laryngology. Supplement.

[11]  F. Hollows,et al.  EAR DISEASE IN RURAL AUSTRALIA , 1979, The Medical journal of Australia.

[12]  L. L. Elliott Performance of children aged 9 to 17 years on a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence material with controlled word predictability. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  B A Schneider,et al.  Infants' detection of speech in noise. , 1981, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[14]  J. Werker,et al.  Developmental changes across childhood in the perception of non-native speech sounds. , 1983, Canadian journal of psychology.

[15]  A K Nábĕlek,et al.  Perception of consonants in reverberation by native and non-native listeners. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  G J McCafferty,et al.  A nine-year study of ear disease in Australian Aboringinal children , 1985, The Journal of Laryngology & Otology.

[17]  C. Mcgilchrist,et al.  Estimation of cumulative illness using cross-sectional data. , 1986, Journal of chronic diseases.

[18]  R. Wagner,et al.  The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. , 1987 .

[19]  M. Reed Speech perception and the discrimination of brief auditory cues in reading disabled children. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[20]  J Jerger,et al.  Effect of linguistic interference on sentence identification. , 1991, Ear and hearing.

[21]  D S Watson,et al.  Ear health of aboriginal primary school children in the Eastern Goldfields Region of Western Australia. , 2010, Australian journal of public health.

[22]  Amanda C. Walley,et al.  The Role of Vocabulary Development in Children′s Spoken Word Recognition and Segmentation Ability , 1993 .

[23]  C S Watson,et al.  Auditory perception, phonological processing, and reading ability/disability. , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  T G Nienhuys,et al.  Onset of Otitis Media in the First Eight Weeks of Life in Aboriginal and Non-Aboriginal Australian Infants , 1995, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[25]  J Boswell,et al.  Presentation of early otitis media in ‘Top End’ Aboriginal infants , 1977, Australian and New Zealand journal of public health.

[26]  W. Hoy,et al.  A health profile of adults in a Northern Territory Aboriginal community, with an emphasis on preventable morbidities , 1977, Australian and New Zealand journal of public health.

[27]  R G Schwartz,et al.  Speech perception and verbal memory in children with and without histories of otitis media. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[28]  Al Yonovitz,et al.  The 'Hearing Aid Effect' in Northern Territory Indigenous Australian Children as Perceived by Their Peers , 2006 .