Behavioural and Hormonal Indicators of Enduring Environmental Stress in Dogs

Animal Welfare 2000, 9: 49-62 Four groups of dogs, which had been subjected to housing conditions of varying quality for years, were assumed to experience different levels of stress. The groups were compared for behavioural and hormonal parameters in order to identifY measures that indicate chronic stress in the dog and which may help to identifY poor welfare in this species. As a standard for comparison, one of the four groups was composed of privately owned dogs; we assumed that chronic stress levels were relatively low in this group (GI). The three remaining groups of dogs (GIl, GIIl and GIV) were kept under conditions of low to relatively high austerity, and had basal urinary ratios of cortisol to creatinine, adrenaline to creatinine and, to a lesser extent, noradrenaline to creatinine, that varied from low to high, respectively. Significant differences (P < 0.05) were found in cortisol to creatinine ratios when comparing GI to GIl, GIll and GIV and when GIl was compared to GIV. Statistical analyses indicated that the mean adrenaline to creatinine ratio in GI differed from that in the remaining groups and that the ratio in GIl differedfrom that in GIll Noradrenaline to creatinine ratios differed significantly only between GI and GIll Dopamine to creatinine ratios and noradrenaline to adrenaline ratios did not differ significantly between groups. When dogs were not disturbed, those that were kept under the most austere conditions typically had high levels of locomotor activity, nosing, urinating and paw lifting. After mild disturbance by a slamming door or in the presence of a researcher these animals reacted actively, with increased locomotor activity, circling and nosing, and they showed high levels of behaviours that have previously been associated with acute stress: body shaking, yawning, ambivalent postures and displacement behaviours. Chronic stress in dogs may be identified by increased paw lifting when animals are not disturbed and by ample behavioural expressions of arousal when they are mildly stimulated. Since some behaviours may occur in contexts not related to stress, behavioural data are easily misinterpreted with regard to chronic stress. Interpretation will only be meaningful when physiological measures such as urinary adrenaline to creatinine ratios and, especially, urinary cortisol to creatinine ratios are also determined.

[1]  J. Mateo,et al.  Influence of housing conditions on beagle behaviour , 1992 .

[2]  R. Anton,et al.  Direct comparison of urinary free cortisol excretion in patients with depression and panic disorder , 1989, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  J. Rushen Stereotypies, aggression and the feeding schedules of tethered sows , 1985 .

[4]  D. F. Morrison,et al.  Multivariate Statistical Methods , 1968 .

[5]  Bonne Beerda,et al.  Chronic Stress in Dogs Subjected to Social and Spatial Restriction. I. Behavioral Responses , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[6]  D. Peeler,et al.  The hippocampus and stress induced 17-OHCS elevations , 1979, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[7]  R. Kathol,et al.  Pathophysiology of HPA axis abnormalities in patients with major depression: an update. , 1989, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  J. Mol,et al.  Assessment of two tests for the diagnosis of canine hyperadrenocorticism , 1988, Veterinary Record.

[9]  M. R. Stoline The Status of Multiple Comparisons: Simultaneous Estimation of all Pairwise Comparisons in One-Way ANOVA Designs , 1981 .

[10]  M. Alvinerie,et al.  Vagally mediated inhibition of acoustic stress-induced cortisol release by orally administered kappa-opioid substances in dogs. , 1989, Endocrinology.

[11]  J. Craig,et al.  Corticosteroids and other indicators of hens' well-being in four laying-house environments. , 1986, Poultry science.

[12]  G. Trancho,et al.  Stress adaptation and adrenal activity in isolated and crowded rats , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[13]  S Levine,et al.  Immediate and proactive effects of controllability and predictability on plasma cortisol responses to shocks in dogs. , 1983, Behavioral neuroscience.

[14]  E. Sassenrath Increased adrenal responsiveness related to social stress in rhesus monkeys , 1970 .

[15]  J. Miller,et al.  Behavioral and glucocorticoid responses of adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to companionship and social separation. , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[16]  Petra A. Mertens,et al.  Effects of group and individual housing on the behavior of kennelled dogs in animal shelters , 1996 .

[17]  Bonne Beerda,et al.  The Use of Saliva Cortisol, Urinary Cortisol, and Catecholamine Measurements for a Noninvasive Assessment of Stress Responses in Dogs , 1996, Hormones and Behavior.

[18]  Trevor B. Poole,et al.  Correlates of pen size and housing conditions on the behaviour of kennelled dogs , 1992 .

[19]  Autonomic response to an experimental psychological stressor in healthy subjects: measurement of sympathetic, parasympathetic, and pituitary-adrenal parameters: test-retest reliability. , 1990, Scandinavian journal of clinical and laboratory investigation.

[20]  D. R. Rager,et al.  Housing and exercise of dogs: effects on behavior, immune function, and cortisol concentration. , 1997, Laboratory animal science.

[21]  M. Alvinerie,et al.  Stimulation of κ opiate receptors in intestinal wall affects stress-induced increase of plasma cortisol in dogs , 1989, Brain Research.

[22]  A. Rijnberk,et al.  Intra- and interindividual variation in urine osmolality and urine specific gravity in healthy pet dogs of various ages. , 1997, Journal of veterinary internal medicine.

[23]  S Cerutti,et al.  Assessment of the neural control of the circulation during psychological stress. , 1991, Journal of the autonomic nervous system.

[24]  R. Nachreiner,et al.  Changes in adrenal cortisol secretion as reflected in the urinary cortisol/creatinine ratio in dogs. , 1990, Domestic animal endocrinology.

[25]  Y. Epstein,et al.  Plasma-cortisol levels in experimental heatstroke in dogs , 1989, International journal of biometeorology.

[26]  S. Cohen,et al.  Stress and immunity in humans: a meta‐analytic review. , 1993, Psychosomatic medicine.

[27]  D. Gann,et al.  Pituitary-adrenal and adrenomedullary responses to noise in awake dogs. , 1990, The American journal of physiology.

[28]  R. Verrier,et al.  Psychologic Stress and Threshold for Repetitive Ventricular Response , 1973, Science.

[29]  J. Brady,et al.  Differential preparatory cardiovascular responses to aversive and appetitive behavioral conditioning , 1972, Conditional reflex.

[30]  D. Palazzolo,et al.  Plasma Thyroxine and Cortisol under Basal Conditions and during Cold Stress in the Aging Dog 1 , 1987, Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine. Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine.

[31]  Bonne Beerda,et al.  Chronic Stress in Dogs Subjected to Social and Spatial Restriction. II. Hormonal and Immunological Responses , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[32]  R. Solomon,et al.  Traumatic avoidance learning: Acquisition in normal dogs. , 1953 .

[33]  P. Obrist,et al.  Blood pressure and cardiac changes during signalled and unsignalled avoidance in dogs , 1977, Physiology & Behavior.

[34]  Bonne Beerda,et al.  Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs , 1998 .

[35]  J. Mitchell,et al.  Cardiac changes during behavioral stress in dogs. , 1979, The American journal of physiology.