Hippocampal Complex Spike Cells do not Change Their Place Fields if the Goal is Moved Within a Cue Controlled Environment

It has been shown that most hippocampal complex‐spike cells fire at specific locations in relation to a set of controlled spatial cues and that they continue to fire in the appropriate places when the cues are removed (O'Keefe and Conway, 1978; O'Keefe and Speakman, 1987). However, the goal remained in a constant position relative to the controlled cues throughout these experiments. It is therefore possible that the unit firing was related to the goal location and not to the controlled cues as was suggested. For example, firing in a particular location might signal the rat's intention to make a particular turn towards the goal from that location and not the location itself. There are many reasons for believing that this is an unlikely class of explanations; nevertheless a test of this hypothesis was undertaken in the present experiment. Nineteen place units were recorded from three rats as they performed the task of finding the goal location. After obtaining an accurate picture of the place fields in relation to the controlled cues and the goal, the location of the goal relative to the cues was moved and the animal retrained to the new goal. After the new goal was being consistently chosen, a second picture of the place fields of the same units was obtained. If the fields were related to the environment created by the cues then they would remain the same during the two stages of the experiment. If they were related to the goal, or to the intended movements towards the goal, then they would rotate relative to the cues. As expected, the majority of place fields remained in the same place in relation to the cues during the two stages of the experiment and were not influenced by the goal relocation. In only two cases was there any suggestion that the place fields changed their location after the goal was moved. It was concluded that the activity of place cells is related to an animal's location in an environment and that the results accord with cognitive map theory of hippocampal function (O'Keefe and Nadel, 1978).

[1]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[2]  J. B. Ranck,et al.  Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. I. Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. , 1973, Experimental neurology.

[3]  J. B. Ranck,et al.  Localization and anatomical identification of theta and complex spike cells in dorsal hippocampal formation of rats , 1975, Experimental Neurology.

[4]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[5]  T W Berger,et al.  Identification of pyramidal cells as the critical elements in hippocampal neuronal plasticity during learning. , 1978, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  A. J. Hill First occurrence of hippocampal spatial firing in a new environment , 1978, Experimental Neurology.

[7]  D. Olton,et al.  Spatial correlates of hippocampal unit activity , 1978, Experimental Neurology.

[8]  J. O’Keefe A review of the hippocampal place cells , 1979, Progress in Neurobiology.

[9]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[10]  John O'Keefe,et al.  On the trail of the hippocampal engram , 1980 .

[11]  P. Best,et al.  Spatial correlates of hippocampal unit activity are altered by lesions of the fornix and entorhinal cortex , 1980, Brain Research.

[12]  Phillip J. Best,et al.  Reliability of the relationship between hippocampal unit activity and sensory-behavioral events in the rat , 1982, Experimental Neurology.

[13]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Single-unit analysis of different hippocampal cell types during classical conditioning of rabbit nictitating membrane response. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  The stereotrode: A new technique for simultaneous isolation of several single units in the central nervous system from multiple unit records , 1983, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[15]  I. Whishaw Cholinergic receptor blockade in the rat impairs locale but not taxon strategies for place navigation in a swimming pool. , 1985, Behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  M. Pontecorvo Memory for a stimulus versus anticipation of a response: Contrasting effects of proactive interference in two delayed comparison tasks , 1985 .

[17]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Neonatal frontal lesions in hamsters impair species-typical behaviors and reduce brain weight and neocortical thickness. , 1985, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  D. Olton,et al.  Mnemonic correlates of unit activity in the hippocampus , 1986, Brain Research.

[19]  M. Kuperstein,et al.  Cue-sampling and goal-approach correlates of hippocampal unit activity in rats performing an odor-discrimination task , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  J. B. Ranck,et al.  Spatial firing patterns of hippocampal complex-spike cells in a fixed environment , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Muller,et al.  The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  R. Hampson,et al.  Hippocampal place cells: stereotypy and plasticity , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Spatial and behavioral correlates of hippocampal neuronal activity. , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.