The Effects of Gender and Argument Strength on the Processing of Word-of-Mouth Communication

This study explores the effects of gender and message argument strength in a unique context: word-of-mouth (WOM) communication. We report the results of an experiment (n=130) in which communicator gender and argument strength are manipulated, and crossed with WOM recipient gender. Our results show that while women describe themselves as being more receptive to WOM in general than males, when specific WOM information is received and processed, both sexes rate that information as equally diagnostic in forming product judgments. As expected, argument strength showed powerful main effects in that stronger arguments led to more positive perceptions of the WOM information’s diagnosticity (usefulness), higher post-WOM brand evaluations, and greater perceived communicator credibility (regardless of the sex of the speaker or the recipient). However, a significant interaction between speaker gender and argument strength was found. Interestingly, male communicators presenting strong arguments fostered the most positive brand attitudes of all the conditions, but male communicators presenting weak arguments led to the lowest brand attitudes. Our results show that positive WOM is most influential on brand evaluations when the communicator’s sex and the WOM recipient’s sex are opposite (i.e., males’ brand evaluations were more influenced by WOM information communicated by a woman and vice versa).

[1]  S. Bem Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. , 1981 .

[2]  Robert E. Smith Integrating Information from Advertising and Trial: Processes and Effects on Consumer Response to Product Information , 1993 .

[3]  Carol T. Miller The role of performance-related similarity in social comparison of abilities: A test of the related attributes hypothesis , 1982 .

[4]  Richard G. Netemeyer,et al.  Measurement of Consumer Susceptibility to Interpersonal Influence , 1989 .

[5]  P. Bone Word-of-mouth effects on short-term and long-term product judgments , 1995 .

[6]  S. J. Gould,et al.  Gift-giving roles and gender self-concepts , 1991 .

[7]  Peter H. Reingen,et al.  Social Ties and Word-of-Mouth Referral Behavior , 1987 .

[8]  Darrel D. Muehling,et al.  The relationship between experimental manipulations and tests of theory in an advertising message , 1993 .

[9]  J. Zaichkowsky Measuring the Involvement Construct , 1985 .

[10]  K. Deaux,et al.  Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. , 1987 .

[11]  Franziska Marquart,et al.  Communication and persuasion : central and peripheral routes to attitude change , 1988 .

[12]  Marsha L. Richins Negative Word-of-Mouth by Dissatisfied Consumers: A Pilot Study , 1983 .

[13]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[14]  D. Leonard-Barton,et al.  Experts as Negative Opinion Leaders in the Diffusion of a Technological Innovation , 1985 .

[15]  Robert E. Smith,et al.  Consumer Processing of Product Trial and the Influence of Prior Advertising: A Structural Modeling Approach , 1998 .

[16]  Barbara Stern,et al.  Sexual identity scale: A new self-assessment measure , 1987 .

[17]  K. File,et al.  Positive Word‐of‐Mouth: Customer Satisfaction and Buyer Behaviour , 1992 .

[18]  M. Hogg,et al.  Numerical distinctiveness, social identity and gender salience , 1990 .

[19]  H. Markus,et al.  Stability and malleability of the self-concept. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  Ezra Stotland,et al.  Generalization of interpersonal similarity. , 1961 .

[21]  Barbara B. Stern,et al.  Gender schema and fashion consciousness , 1989 .

[22]  Ellen Garbarino,et al.  GENDER DIFFERENCES IN THE PERCEIVED RISK OF BUYING ONLINE AND THE EFFECTS OF RECEIVING A SITE RECOMMENDATION , 2004 .

[23]  R. Helmreich,et al.  Ratings of self and peers on sex role attributes and their relation to self-esteem and conceptions of masculinity and femininity. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Lawrence Feick,et al.  Preference heterogeneity and coorientation as determinants of perceived informational influence , 1989 .

[25]  Peter D. Bennett,et al.  The Role of Confidence in Understanding and Predicting Buyers' Attitudes and Purchase Intentions , 1975 .

[26]  Lawrence Feick,et al.  The Role of Interpersonal Sources in External Search: an Informational Perspective , 1984 .

[27]  Alice M. Tybout,et al.  Designing Research for Application , 1981 .

[28]  P. Herr,et al.  Effects of Word-of-Mouth and Product-Attribute Information on Persuasion: An Accessibility-Diagnosticity Perspective , 1991 .

[29]  C. Areni,et al.  Reexamining Masculinity, Femininity,and Gender Identity Scales , 1999 .

[30]  Roobina Ohanian Construction and Validation of a Scale to Measure Celebrity Endorsers' Perceived Expertise, Trustworthiness, and Attractiveness , 1990 .

[31]  S. Bem The measurement of psychological androgyny. , 1974, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[32]  J. Gentry,et al.  Gender Schema Theory As a Predictor of Ad Recall , 1984 .

[33]  D. Kuhn,et al.  Sex role concepts of two- and three-year-olds. , 1978, Child development.

[34]  K. B. Murray A Test of Services Marketing Theory: Consumer Information Acquisition Activities , 1991 .

[35]  Tommy E. Whittler The Effects of Actors' Race in Commercial Advertising: Review and Extension , 1991 .

[36]  C. Vogt,et al.  The Effects of Integrating Advertising and Negative Word‐of‐Mouth Communications on Message Processing and Response , 1995 .

[37]  H. Markus,et al.  Self-schemas and gender. , 1982 .