Serotonergic modulation of response inhibition and re‐engagement? Results of a study in healthy human volunteers

Cognitive functions dependent on the prefrontal cortex, such as the ability to suppress behavior (response inhibition) and initiate a new one (response re‐engagement) is important in the activities of daily life. Central serotonin (5‐HT) function is thought to be a critical component of these cognitive functions. In recent studies, 5‐HT failed to affect stop‐signal reaction time (SSRT), a fundamental process in behavioral inhibition. We were interested if response inhibition and re‐engagement are influenced through central 5‐HT activity as mediated via the 5‐HT transporter.

[1]  P. L. McCormack,et al.  Escitalopram , 2010, CNS drugs.

[2]  M. Boecker,et al.  Differential effects of escitalopram on attention: a placebo-controlled, double-blind cross-over study , 2009, Psychopharmacology.

[3]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  T. Robbins,et al.  Serotonin Depletion Impairs Waiting but not Stop-Signal Reaction Time in Rats: Implications for Theories of the Role of 5-HT in Behavioral Inhibition , 2009, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[5]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neurobehavioral mechanisms of impulsivity: Fronto-striatal systems and functional neurochemistry , 2008, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[6]  T. Robbins,et al.  The neuropsychopharmacology of action inhibition: cross-species translation of the stop-signal and go/no-go tasks , 2008, Psychopharmacology.

[7]  Xiao-Hua Zhou,et al.  Statistical Methods for Meta‐Analysis , 2008 .

[8]  Saori C. Tanaka,et al.  Low-Serotonin Levels Increase Delayed Reward Discounting in Humans , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  T. Robbins,et al.  Serotoninergic regulation of emotional and behavioural control processes , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  T. Robbins Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[12]  Matthias L. Schroeter,et al.  Prefrontal brain activation during stop-signal response inhibition: An event-related functional near-infrared spectroscopy study , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  C. Yokoyama,et al.  Relationship between limbic and cortical 5-HT neurotransmission and acquisition and reversal learning in a go/no-go task in rats , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behaviorally and neurochemically specific. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neurochemical Modulation of Response Inhibition and Probabilistic Learning in Humans , 2006, Science.

[16]  F. Larsen,et al.  The Pharmacokinetics of Escitalopram After Oral and Intravenous Administration of Single and Multiple Doses to Healthy Subjects , 2005, Journal of clinical pharmacology.

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop signal response inhibition is not modulated by tryptophan depletion or the serotonin transporter polymorphism in healthy volunteers: implications for the 5-HT theory of impulsivity , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[18]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  The Effect of Citalopram Pretreatment on Neuronal Responses to Neuropsychological Tasks in Normal Volunteers: An fMRI Study , 2005, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[19]  T. Robbins,et al.  The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[20]  Trevor W Robbins,et al.  Interactions between Serotonin and Dopamine in the Control of Impulsive Choice in Rats: Therapeutic Implications for Impulse Control Disorders , 2005, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[21]  T. Robbins,et al.  Prefrontal Serotonin Depletion Affects Reversal Learning But Not Attentional Set Shifting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  M. Pinza,et al.  A cortical GABA–5HT interaction in the mechanism of action of the antidepressant trazodone , 2004, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[23]  M. Rieger,et al.  Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[24]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibitory control in rats performing a stop-signal reaction-time task: effects of lesions of the medial striatum and d-amphetamine. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  T. Robbins,et al.  Global 5-HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay-discounting task in rats , 2003, Psychopharmacology.

[26]  Katya Rubia,et al.  Motor Timing Deficits in Community and Clinical Boys with Hyperactive Behavior: The Effect of Methylphenidate on Motor Timing , 2003, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[27]  M. Rieger,et al.  Inhibition of ongoing responses following frontal, nonfrontal, and basal ganglia lesions. , 2003, Neuropsychology.

[28]  D. Dougherty,et al.  Laboratory-Measured Aggressive Behavior of Women: Acute Tryptophan Depletion and Augmentation , 2002, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[29]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.

[30]  J. Bjork,et al.  Differential Behavioral Effects of Plasma Tryptophan Depletion and Loading in Aggressive and Nonaggressive Men , 2000, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[31]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Functional frontalisation with age: mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories with fMRI , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[32]  C. Montigny,et al.  Serotonin and Drug-Induced Therapeutic Responses in Major Depression, Obsessive–Compulsive and Panic Disorders , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[33]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: a study with functional MRI. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[34]  J. Hennig,et al.  Psychobiological Differences Between the Aggression and Psychoticism Dimension , 1999, Pharmacopsychiatry.

[35]  J. Hennig,et al.  Personality Correlates of +/- Pindolol Induced Decreases in Prolactin , 1998, Pharmacopsychiatry.

[36]  J. Pearce,et al.  Acquisition of Conditioned Inhibition in Rats is Impaired by Ablation of Serotoninergic Pathways , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  C Kaernbach,et al.  Simple adaptive testing with the weighted up-down method , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple thoughts and actions: II. Stop-signal studies of repetition priming. , 1985 .

[39]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[40]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. , 1966 .

[41]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal reaction-time task performance: role of prefrontal cortex and subthalamic nucleus. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  W J Riedel,et al.  Serotonin and human cognitive performance. , 2006, Current pharmaceutical design.

[43]  W. Riedel,et al.  Effects of 5-HT on Memory and the Hippocampus: Model and Data , 2006, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[44]  M. Lader,et al.  The effects of citalopram in single and repeated doses and with alcohol on physiological and psychological measures in healthy subjects , 2004, European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology.

[45]  K. Goa,et al.  Escitalopram , 2003, CNS drugs.

[46]  T. Robbins,et al.  From arousal to cognition: the integrative position of the prefrontal cortex. , 2000, Progress in brain research.

[47]  R. A. Littman Inhibition: History and Meaning in the Sciences of Mind and Brain. , 1993 .

[48]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Dependence and Independence in Responding to Double Stimulation : A Comparison of Stop , Change , and Dual-Task Paradigms , 1986 .

[49]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  How to Stop and Change a Response: the Role of Goal Activation in Multitasking , 2022 .

[50]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[51]  G. Logan,et al.  Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[52]  American Psychological Association, Inc. On the Ability to Inhibit Simple and Choice Reaction Time Responses: A Model and a Method , 2022 .

[53]  G. Logan American Psychological Association, Inc.>n the Ability to Inhibit Simple Thoughts and Actions: I. Stop-Signal Studies of Decision and Memory , 2022 .