Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. Forster,et al. Unconscious cognition isn’t that smart: Modulation of masked repetition priming effect in the word naming task , 2008, Cognition.
[2] Matthew J. C. Crump,et al. Short article: The flexibility of context-specific control: Evidence for context-driven generalization of item-specific control settings , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[3] G R Grice,et al. Stimulus intensity and response evocation. , 1968, Psychological review.
[4] Frederick Verbruggen,et al. Stimulus- and response-conflict-induced cognitive control in the flanker task , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] James R. Schmidt,et al. The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[6] T. Egner,et al. Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[7] Chris Blais,et al. Behavioral and Neural Evidence for Item-specific Performance Monitoring , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] Wim Notebaert,et al. Adaptation by binding: a learning account of cognitive control , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[9] P. Holland. Occasion Setting in Pavlovian Conditioning , 1992 .
[10] Kenneth I Forster,et al. Dynamic adaptation to history of trial difficulty explains the effect of congruency proportion on masked priming. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[11] L. Jacoby,et al. Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[12] W. Perlstein,et al. Conflict adaptation and cognitive control adjustments following traumatic brain injury , 2009, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[13] Lidewij L. van Duren,et al. On the robustness of the additive factors stage structure in blocked and mixed choice reaction designs. , 1988, Acta psychologica.
[14] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[15] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Unconsciously Triggered Conflict Adaptation , 2010, PloS one.
[16] T. Goschke,et al. Implicit learning of semantic category sequences: response-independent acquisition of abstract sequential regularities. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[17] William M. Perlstein,et al. Neural time course of conflict adaptation effects on the Stroop task , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[18] T. Egner,et al. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Information and expression of simultaneous and backward associations: Implications for contiguity theory☆ , 1988 .
[20] D. Lowe,et al. Selective and divided Attention in a Stroop task. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.
[21] David Hume. A Treatise of Human Nature: Being an Attempt to introduce the experimental Method of Reasoning into Moral Subjects , 1972 .
[22] Aziz M. Ulug,et al. Parametric manipulation of conflict and response competition using rapid mixed-trial event-related fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.
[23] J. T. Mordkoff. Observation: Three reasons to avoid having half of the trials be congruent in a four-alternative forced-choice experiment on sequential modulation , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[24] J. de Houwer. A Conceptual and Theoretical Analysis of Evaluative Conditioning , 2007, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.
[25] James R. Schmidt,et al. Does Temporal Contiguity Moderate Contingency Learning in a Speeded Performance Task? , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[26] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[27] J. Cohen,et al. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.
[28] P. Merikle,et al. Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual processes. , 1986, Canadian journal of psychology.
[29] L. Jacoby,et al. Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[30] W. Notebaert,et al. Dissociating conflict adaptation from feature integration: a multiple regression approach. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] Sachiko Kinoshita,et al. Priming and attentional control of lexical and sublexical pathways in naming: a reevaluation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[32] Grice Gr,et al. Stimulus intensity and response evocation. , 1968 .
[33] E. Kandel,et al. Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activity in the Amygdala , 2006, Neuron.
[34] T. Egner,et al. Going, Going, Gone: Characterizing the Time-Course of Congruency Sequence Effects , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[35] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[36] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. Strategies and automaticity. I: Basic findings and conceptual framework , 1994 .
[37] J. Houwer. Why the Cognitive Approach in Psychology Would Profit From a Functional Approach and Vice Versa , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[38] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. Strategies and automaticity: II. Dynamic aspects of strategy adjustment. , 1994 .
[39] L. Jacoby,et al. Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] Derek Besner,et al. Contingency learning without awareness: Evidence for implicit control , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.
[41] R. Cabeza,et al. Imaging Cognition: An Empirical Review of PET Studies with Normal Subjects , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[42] Derek Besner,et al. Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. , 2007, Psychological review.
[43] Michael C. Mozer,et al. A Rational Analysis of Cognitive Control in a Speeded Discrimination Task , 2001, NIPS.
[44] G. Grice,et al. STIMULUS INTENSITY EFFECTS DEPEND UPON THE TYPE OF EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN. , 1964, Psychological review.
[45] J. de Houwer,et al. Retrieval of incidental stimulus-response associations as a source of negative priming. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[46] Peter E. Clayson,et al. Conflict adaptation and sequential trial effects: Support for the conflict monitoring theory , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[47] James R. Schmidt. The Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) model: dissociating contingency and conflict adaptation in the item-specific proportion congruent paradigm. , 2013, Acta psychologica.
[48] Tobias Teichert,et al. The dorsal medial frontal cortex is sensitive to time on task, not response conflict or error likelihood , 2011, NeuroImage.
[49] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[50] D. L. Kohfeld. Stimulus intensity and adaptation level as determinants of simple reaction time. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[51] Nicholas Wymbs,et al. Neural correlates of conflict processing , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] N. Atalay,et al. Can contingency learning alone account for item-specific control? Evidence from within- and between-language ISPC effects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[53] C Alain,et al. Event-related neural activity associated with the Stroop task. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[54] T. Egner. Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[55] J. R. Simon,et al. Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.
[56] A. Michotte. The perception of causality , 1963 .
[57] R. Shillcock,et al. Proceedings of the Twenty-Sixth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1998 .
[58] Heather A. Wilk,et al. Brain regions associated with moment-to-moment adjustments in control and stable task-set maintenance , 2012, NeuroImage.
[59] K. Hutchison,et al. The Interactive Effects of Listwide Control, Item-based Control, and Working Memory Capacity on Stroop Performance and Holly Vander Vos for Scheduling and Running Participants for This Study. I Am Also Grateful To , 2022 .
[60] B. Hommel. Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .
[61] James R. Schmidt,et al. Contingency learning with evaluative stimuli: testing the generality of contingency learning in a performance paradigm. , 2012, Experimental psychology.
[62] J. Healey. The Preparation: , 2019, Walter Baade.
[63] J. Mordkoff. 18. Selective attention and internal constraints: there is more to the flanker effect than biased contingencies , 1996 .
[64] G. Humphreys,et al. Sustained vs. transient cognitive control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation , 2010, Cognition.
[65] R. West,et al. Effects of task context and fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting performance of the Stroop task , 2000, Brain Research.
[66] Peter E. Clayson,et al. Cognitive control in mild traumatic brain injury: conflict monitoring and conflict adaptation. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[67] J. Houwer. A Conceptual and Theoretical Analysis of Evaluative Conditioning , 2007 .
[68] M. McDaniel,et al. Revealing list-level control in the Stroop task by uncovering its benefits and a cost. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[69] Derek Besner,et al. Contingency learning and unlearning in the blink of an eye: A resource dependent process , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.
[70] Matthew J. C. Crump,et al. The context-specific proportion congruent Stroop effect: Location as a contextual cue , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[71] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[72] Norbert M. Seel,et al. Encyclopedia of the sciences of learning , 2012 .
[73] E. Awh,et al. Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[74] Tobias Egner,et al. Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Mediates Individual Differences in Conflict-driven Cognitive Control , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[75] W. Notebaert,et al. Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. , 2008, Psychological review.
[76] L. Jacoby,et al. Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[77] Michael C. Mozer,et al. Control of Response Initiation: Mechanisms of Adaptation to Recent Experience , 2004 .
[78] James R. Schmidt,et al. Adding the goal to learn strengthens learning in an unintentional learning task , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[79] E. Hazeltine,et al. Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[80] S. Los,et al. On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .
[81] J. Bugg,et al. List-wide control is not entirely elusive: Evidence from picture–word Stroop , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[82] James R. Schmidt,et al. Now you see it, now you don't: controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions eliminates the Gratton effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[83] G. Logan,et al. Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention , 1996 .
[84] Shaun R. Patel,et al. Human Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Neurons Mediate Ongoing Behavioral Adaptation , 2012, Nature.
[85] E. Hazeltine,et al. Domain-specific conflict adaptation without feature repetitions , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[86] B. Hommel,et al. A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.
[87] M. J. Billington,et al. The Deadline model for simple reaction times , 1972 .