Perceptual load influences auditory space perception in the ventriloquist aftereffect
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Salvador Soto-Faraco,et al. Searching for audiovisual correspondence in multiple speaker scenarios , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] D. Senkowski,et al. The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[3] B. Shinn-Cunningham,et al. Reference Frame of the Ventriloquism Aftereffect , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] Ilja Frissen,et al. Visual recalibration of auditory spatial perception: two separate neural circuits for perceptual learning , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[5] S. Vecera,et al. Perceptual load modulates attentional capture by abrupt onsets , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[6] S. Fairhall,et al. Spatial attention can modulate audiovisual integration at multiple cortical and subcortical sites , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[7] Eduardo Martínez-Montes,et al. Hemispheric modulations of alpha-band power reflect the rightward shift in attention induced by enhanced attentional load , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[8] Scott Sinnett,et al. The effect of attention on the illusory capture of motion in bimodal stimuli , 2008, Brain Research.
[9] John J. Foxe,et al. Look who's talking: The deployment of visuo-spatial attention during multisensory speech processing under noisy environmental conditions , 2008, NeuroImage.
[10] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Attention Facilitates Multiple Stimulus Features in Parallel in Human Visual Cortex , 2008, Current Biology.
[11] G. Recanzone,et al. The biological basis of audition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[12] Salvador Soto-Faraco,et al. Attention to touch weakens audiovisual speech integration , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] William E O'Neill,et al. Auditory Spatial Perception Dynamically Realigns with Changing Eye Position , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14] John Duncan,et al. Modulation of spatial bias in the dual task paradigm: Evidence from patients with unilateral parietal lesions and controls , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[15] Isabelle Viaud-Delmon,et al. Ventriloquism aftereffects occur in the rear hemisphere , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.
[16] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Visual search for a target changing in synchrony with an auditory signal , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[17] P. Bertelson,et al. The aftereffects of ventriloquism: Patterns of spatial generalization , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[18] C. Dodds,et al. Rightward shift in spatial awareness with declining alertness , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[19] Gregory McCarthy,et al. Graded Visual Attention Modulates Brain Responses Evoked by Task-irrelevant Auditory Pitch Changes , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] R. Zatorre,et al. Attention to simultaneous unrelated auditory and visual events: behavioral and neural correlates. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[21] Daniel Senkowski,et al. Multisensory processing and oscillatory gamma responses: effects of spatial selective attention , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] Marty G. Woldorff,et al. Selective Attention and Multisensory Integration: Multiple Phases of Effects on the Evoked Brain Activity , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[23] R. Dolan,et al. Attentional load and sensory competition in human vision: modulation of fMRI responses by load at fixation during task-irrelevant stimulation in the peripheral visual field. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[24] R. Campbell,et al. Audiovisual Integration of Speech Falters under High Attention Demands , 2005, Current Biology.
[25] A. King,et al. Multisensory Integration: Strategies for Synchronization , 2005, Current Biology.
[26] E. Macaluso,et al. Multisensory spatial interactions: a window onto functional integration in the human brain , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.
[27] N. Lavie. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] M. Pinsk,et al. Push-pull mechanism of selective attention in human extrastriate cortex. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[29] Alice J. O'Toole,et al. Repetition-based familiarity improves person recognition in novel contexts , 2004 .
[30] Leila Montaser-Kouhsari,et al. Attentional modulation of adaptation to illusory lines. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[31] D. Burr,et al. The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.
[32] Paul Bertelson,et al. The aftereffects of ventriloquism: are they sound-frequency specific? , 2003, Acta psychologica.
[33] C. Spence,et al. Multisensory Integration: Maintaining the Perception of Synchrony , 2003, Current Biology.
[34] J. Lewald. Rapid adaptation to auditory-visual spatial disparity. , 2002, Learning & memory.
[35] W. Teder-Sälejärvi,et al. Multisensory integration and crossmodal attention effects in the human brain. , 2001, Science.
[36] P. Bertelson,et al. The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of automatic visual attention , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[37] Y. Rossetti,et al. Visual extinction in oculocentric coordinates: A selective bias in dividing attention between hemifields , 2000 .
[38] G. Recanzone. Rapidly induced auditory plasticity: the ventriloquism aftereffect. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[39] Tom Manly,et al. Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[40] C D Frith,et al. Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.
[41] N. Lavie,et al. On the Efficiency of Visual Selective Attention: Efficient Visual Search Leads to Inefficient Distractor Rejection , 1997 .
[42] Jörg Lewald,et al. Eye-position effects in directional hearing , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.
[43] Simon R. Oldfield,et al. Acuity of Sound Localisation: A Topography of Auditory Space. I. Normal Hearing Conditions , 1984, Perception.
[44] H. McGurk,et al. Hearing lips and seeing voices , 1976, Nature.
[45] P. Bertelson,et al. The After-Effects of Ventriloquism , 1974, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[46] L. K. Canon. Intermodality inconsistency of input and directed attention as determinants of the nature of adaptation. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.
[47] J. Theeuwes,et al. Pip and pop: nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] Charles Spence,et al. Perceptual load affects exogenous spatial orienting while working memory load does not , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] P. Bertelson,et al. The aftereffects of ventriloquism: generalization across sound-frequencies. , 2005, Acta psychologica.
[50] P. Bertelson,et al. The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of deliberate visual attention , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[51] Jason B. Mattingley,et al. Extinction as a paradigm measure of attentional bias and restricted capacity. , 1997 .
[52] Peter Thier,et al. Parietal Lobe Contributions to Orientation in 3D Space , 1997 .
[53] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[54] D. LaBerge,et al. Theory of attentional operations in shape identification. , 1989 .