Perceptual load influences auditory space perception in the ventriloquist aftereffect

[1]  Salvador Soto-Faraco,et al.  Searching for audiovisual correspondence in multiple speaker scenarios , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  D. Senkowski,et al.  The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  B. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Reference Frame of the Ventriloquism Aftereffect , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Ilja Frissen,et al.  Visual recalibration of auditory spatial perception: two separate neural circuits for perceptual learning , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  S. Vecera,et al.  Perceptual load modulates attentional capture by abrupt onsets , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  S. Fairhall,et al.  Spatial attention can modulate audiovisual integration at multiple cortical and subcortical sites , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  Eduardo Martínez-Montes,et al.  Hemispheric modulations of alpha-band power reflect the rightward shift in attention induced by enhanced attentional load , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Scott Sinnett,et al.  The effect of attention on the illusory capture of motion in bimodal stimuli , 2008, Brain Research.

[9]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Look who's talking: The deployment of visuo-spatial attention during multisensory speech processing under noisy environmental conditions , 2008, NeuroImage.

[10]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Attention Facilitates Multiple Stimulus Features in Parallel in Human Visual Cortex , 2008, Current Biology.

[11]  G. Recanzone,et al.  The biological basis of audition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[12]  Salvador Soto-Faraco,et al.  Attention to touch weakens audiovisual speech integration , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  William E O'Neill,et al.  Auditory Spatial Perception Dynamically Realigns with Changing Eye Position , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  John Duncan,et al.  Modulation of spatial bias in the dual task paradigm: Evidence from patients with unilateral parietal lesions and controls , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Isabelle Viaud-Delmon,et al.  Ventriloquism aftereffects occur in the rear hemisphere , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[16]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Visual search for a target changing in synchrony with an auditory signal , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  P. Bertelson,et al.  The aftereffects of ventriloquism: Patterns of spatial generalization , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  C. Dodds,et al.  Rightward shift in spatial awareness with declining alertness , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  Gregory McCarthy,et al.  Graded Visual Attention Modulates Brain Responses Evoked by Task-irrelevant Auditory Pitch Changes , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Attention to simultaneous unrelated auditory and visual events: behavioral and neural correlates. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Daniel Senkowski,et al.  Multisensory processing and oscillatory gamma responses: effects of spatial selective attention , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Marty G. Woldorff,et al.  Selective Attention and Multisensory Integration: Multiple Phases of Effects on the Evoked Brain Activity , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Dolan,et al.  Attentional load and sensory competition in human vision: modulation of fMRI responses by load at fixation during task-irrelevant stimulation in the peripheral visual field. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  R. Campbell,et al.  Audiovisual Integration of Speech Falters under High Attention Demands , 2005, Current Biology.

[25]  A. King,et al.  Multisensory Integration: Strategies for Synchronization , 2005, Current Biology.

[26]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Multisensory spatial interactions: a window onto functional integration in the human brain , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[27]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  M. Pinsk,et al.  Push-pull mechanism of selective attention in human extrastriate cortex. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Alice J. O'Toole,et al.  Repetition-based familiarity improves person recognition in novel contexts , 2004 .

[30]  Leila Montaser-Kouhsari,et al.  Attentional modulation of adaptation to illusory lines. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[31]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[32]  Paul Bertelson,et al.  The aftereffects of ventriloquism: are they sound-frequency specific? , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[33]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory Integration: Maintaining the Perception of Synchrony , 2003, Current Biology.

[34]  J. Lewald Rapid adaptation to auditory-visual spatial disparity. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[35]  W. Teder-Sälejärvi,et al.  Multisensory integration and crossmodal attention effects in the human brain. , 2001, Science.

[36]  P. Bertelson,et al.  The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of automatic visual attention , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Visual extinction in oculocentric coordinates: A selective bias in dividing attention between hemifields , 2000 .

[38]  G. Recanzone Rapidly induced auditory plasticity: the ventriloquism aftereffect. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  Tom Manly,et al.  Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[41]  N. Lavie,et al.  On the Efficiency of Visual Selective Attention: Efficient Visual Search Leads to Inefficient Distractor Rejection , 1997 .

[42]  Jörg Lewald,et al.  Eye-position effects in directional hearing , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[43]  Simon R. Oldfield,et al.  Acuity of Sound Localisation: A Topography of Auditory Space. I. Normal Hearing Conditions , 1984, Perception.

[44]  H. McGurk,et al.  Hearing lips and seeing voices , 1976, Nature.

[45]  P. Bertelson,et al.  The After-Effects of Ventriloquism , 1974, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[46]  L. K. Canon Intermodality inconsistency of input and directed attention as determinants of the nature of adaptation. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[47]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Pip and pop: nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  Charles Spence,et al.  Perceptual load affects exogenous spatial orienting while working memory load does not , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  P. Bertelson,et al.  The aftereffects of ventriloquism: generalization across sound-frequencies. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[50]  P. Bertelson,et al.  The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of deliberate visual attention , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[51]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Extinction as a paradigm measure of attentional bias and restricted capacity. , 1997 .

[52]  Peter Thier,et al.  Parietal Lobe Contributions to Orientation in 3D Space , 1997 .

[53]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[54]  D. LaBerge,et al.  Theory of attentional operations in shape identification. , 1989 .